The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Recognition memory is impaired in patients with schizophrenia, as they rely largely on item familiarity, rather than conscious recollection, to make mnemonic decisions. False recognition of novel items (foils) is increased in schizophrenia and may relate to this deficit in conscious recollection. By studying pictures of the target word during encoding, healthy adults can suppress false recognition. This study examined the effect of pictorial encoding on subsequent recognition of repeated foils in patients with schizophrenia. METHOD: The study included 40 patients with schizophrenia and 32 healthy comparison subjects. After incidental encoding of 60 words or pictures, subjects were tested for recognition of target items intermixed with 60 new foils. These new foils were subsequently repeated following either a two- or 24-word delay. Subjects were instructed to label these repeated foils as new and not to mistake them for old target words. RESULTS: Schizophrenic patients showed greater overall false recognition of repeated foils. The rate of false recognition of repeated foils was lower after picture encoding than after word encoding. Despite higher levels of false recognition of repeated new items, patients and comparison subjects demonstrated a similar degree of false recognition suppression after picture, as compared to word, encoding. CONCLUSIONS: Patients with schizophrenia displayed greater false recognition of repeated foils than comparison subjects, suggesting both a decrement of item- (or source-) specific recollection and a consequent reliance on familiarity in schizophrenia. Despite these deficits, presenting pictorial information at encoding allowed schizophrenic subjects to suppress false recognition to a similar degree as the comparison group, implying the intact use of a high-level cognitive strategy in this population.

While patients with schizophrenia show impairment in many cognitive processes (1, 2), not all of these domains are affected to a similar degree. Recent work, particularly in the realm of memory, has focused on these differential deficits, as they may inform our understanding of the underlying pathophysiology leading to schizophrenia (3, 4).

Although patients with schizophrenia show a decline in explicit memory, implicit memory (i.e., procedural learning and priming) remains relatively unaffected (58). Similarly, patients with schizophrenia demonstrate impairment in memory processes requiring conscious recollection, with relative preservation of mnemonic processes based on familiarity (9, 10).

To examine the separate influences of conscious recollection and familiarity on memory performance in schizophrenia, we used an old/new recognition memory experiment in which participants must distinguish previously studied words from new words (foils) (1115). The new words are then repeated, with a variable lag between their first and second occurrence. Participants are instructed to respond “old” only to studied words and “new” to new words (even when they repeat), being careful not to mistake the repeating new foils for old items.

Following sufficiently long lags (e.g., after 24 or 48 intervening items), healthy adults will falsely recognize these repeated foils as study list items at a significantly higher rate than they falsely recognize once-presented new words (11). As these repeated new items produce a similar degree of familiarity as previously encountered study list items, relying on familiarity to make old/new judgments is not a fruitful strategy. Recollection of specific details associated with the prior occurrence of a word is required to counter any familiarity produced by the second presentation of the new word on the test (12). This type of errant response based on familiarity is even more pronounced in healthy elderly subjects, even when the lag between repetitions is as short as four intervening items (11, 12, 14, 16).

In addition to the processes of recollection and familiarity, there is a strategic retrieval process, called the distinctiveness heuristic, that also influences the degree of false recognition (11, 15, 17–19). This heuristic refers to a mode of responding influenced by the expectation of recollecting vivid details about previously experienced events. An inability to recall distinct information about a particular item suggests that the item was not seen previously and can be categorized as new. For example, studying pictures (instead of words) produces a dramatic reduction in the rate of false recognition of repeated new words in both younger and older adults (11).

The distinctiveness heuristic is categorized as a metacognitive process, as it is presumed to require introspection of personal memory expectations (e.g., “If I had seen a picture of xx, I think I would remember it.”) (19). As patients with schizophrenia demonstrate impaired insight into their own mental and physical states (20, 21) and have been shown to exhibit abnormal biases in memory attribution (22, 23), we were particularly interested in assessing the ability of patients with schizophrenia to utilize this type of heuristic.

We therefore tested two main hypotheses:

1. Patients with schizophrenia will demonstrate poor item-specific recollection and will rely on familiarity to make old/new decisions, leading to a greater degree of false recognition of repeated foils than seen in healthy adults.

2. Patients with schizophrenia will not utilize the distinctiveness heuristic and will therefore not demonstrate the normal pattern of false recognition suppression after picture encoding, as compared to word encoding.

Method

Subjects

Forty male patients with DSM-IV-defined schizophrenia were recruited from our outpatient clinic in Boston. Confirmation of diagnosis and determination of schizophrenic subtype was made for all patients by clinic psychiatrists, who administered the psychotic disorders module of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders, Patient Edition (SCID-P) (24). All patients were taking a stable dose of antipsychotic medication (12 were taking conventional neuroleptics, 27 were taking atypical neuroleptics, and one was taking lithium monotherapy) and were not withdrawn from their medication for the purposes of the study. The mean dose of medication was equivalent to 557 mg of chlorpromazine. In addition, six of the patients were taking an anticholinergic medication (four were taking benztropine, and two were taking trihexyphenidyl) at the time of study participation. Patients were excluded if they had a history of significant neurological illness (seizure disorder, head trauma, stroke) or if they met criteria for alcohol or other substance abuse within the past 3 months.

Thirty-two age-matched male subjects, recruited by means of advertisement from the local community, served as a comparison group. Comparison subjects were free of any axis I psychiatric condition as determined by the SCID-P (administered by A.P.W.) and did not have a history of major medical or neurological illness or of substance abuse. None of the comparison subjects was taking psychotropic medication. One of the 32 comparison subjects had a first-degree relative (brother) with a history of schizophrenia.

Comparison subjects had a higher level of formal education and a greater mean parental level of education than the schizophrenia group (Table 1). There were no significant within-group differences in age, education, parental education, duration of illness, or symptom severity (as defined by the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale [25]) between subjects assigned to the picture- or word-encoding conditions.

Before enrollment of subjects, the protocol was approved by the institutional review boards of both the Massachusetts General Hospital and the state Department of Mental Health. After a complete description of the study, informed, written consent was obtained from all participants.

Materials and Study Design

The experimental paradigm was an old/new recognition memory test (1114, 26). The stimuli consisted of 120 line drawings or their corresponding verbal labels (27). Sixty items were studied, either as pictures or words, and were then seen as old items at the time of the test. The other 60 items served as new foils at the time of the test, with half of these items repeating at either a two-word or a 24-word delay (Figure 1).

Participants were alternatively assigned to either the picture- or word-encoding condition. Stimuli were presented on a computer screen, concurrent with auditory presentation of the item name by means of headphones. Subjects were told to indicate the number of syllables (one or two) in the study item by using a self-paced button press and were unaware of the subsequent memory test.

In the test phase, which occurred immediately after the items were studied, all items appeared in word format without auditory presentation, regardless of study condition. Participants were informed that they would now see old words (i.e., items seen during the syllable-counting phase) and new words (i.e., items not seen previously) and would be asked to distinguish between the two by using a self-paced button response. They were also told that the new items would repeat so that they would be seen twice. They were instructed to continue to consider these repeating new words as new, as they were not encountered during the syllable-counting phase of the experiment.

Statistical Analysis

Correct recognition of old items (hit rate) and the false characterization of new foils as old during their initial presentation (false-alarm rate) were analyzed by using a two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) (group: schizophrenia versus comparison; encoding condition: picture versus word). Old/new discrimination accuracy and response bias were measured with the signal detection parameters of discrimination (d′) and bias (C), respectively (28), on the basis of the hit rate and false-alarm rate of each subject. As hit rates of 0 or 1 lead to undefined values in these analyses, the data were transformed in standard fashion by adding 0.5 to each individual’s hit total and dividing by N + 1 (rather than N) (29). Values of d′ and C were then also analyzed by using a two-way ANOVA (group and encoding condition).

To determine the degree of false recognition of the repeated foils, false recognition rates after the two-word and 24-word delay were first corrected by subtracting out the baseline rate of false recognition of new words at initial presentation. This corrected rate of false recognition was then analyzed by using repeated measures ANOVA, with group and encoding condition as between-subject factors and length of delay (two-word versus 24-word) as a within-subject factor.

To examine the potential confound of mean parental education on memory performance, we ran a simple linear regression analysis of mean parental education versus overall discrimination accuracy (d′) and corrected rate of false recognition. We examined the potential confound of encoding performance (syllable counting) on subsequent memory in a similar fashion. We studied the potential impact of medication status on performance in two ways: 1) by means of a simple linear regression analysis of neuroleptic dose (in chlorpromazine equivalents) versus d′ and corrected rate of false recognition, and 2) by using t tests to compare these memory performance statistics between patients taking conventional neuroleptics and those taking atypical neuroleptics and between patients taking anticholinergic medications and those who were not.

Results

Hit Rate, False-Alarm Rate, and Signal Detection Analyses

Correct recognition of previously studied items (hit rate) was greater in comparison subjects (mean=0.61, SD=0.19) than patients with schizophrenia (mean=0.45, SD=0.23) (F=9.81, df=1, 68, p<0.005) and tended to be higher after word encoding (mean=0.58, SD=0.20) than after picture encoding (mean=0.47, SD=0.25) (F=3.59, df=1, 68, p=0.06) (Table 2). There was no interaction of group and encoding condition.

The false recognition of new foils at their initial presentation (false-alarm rate) did not differ significantly between comparison and patient groups (mean=0.19, SD=0.14, and mean=0.14, SD=0.13, respectively). The false-alarm rate was higher after word (mean=0.21, SD=0.13) than picture (mean=0.12, SD=0.13) encoding (F=8.15, df=1, 68, p<0.01), without an interaction of group and encoding condition.

Signal detection analyses indicated that comparison subjects were better at discriminating old from new items, as evidenced by a higher level of d′ (1.30 versus 1.07) (F=4.01, df=1, 68, p=0.05). Encoding condition had no significant effect on old/new discriminability. Schizophrenic subjects had a significantly more conservative response bias (i.e., a greater reluctance to call items old), as evidenced by the higher criterion threshold (C=0.67 versus 0.35) (F=6.37, df=1, 68, p<0.05). Subjects in both groups were more conservative in their responses after picture encoding (C=0.71) than after word encoding (C=0.35) (F=8.04, df=1, 68, p<0.01).

False Recognition of Repeated Foils

Schizophrenic subjects showed greater false recognition of repeated foils (corrected for baseline rate of false recognition) (mean=0.18, SD=0.16) than the comparison subjects (mean=0.09, SD=0.13) (F=11.28, df=1, 68, p<0.005). The rate of false recognition of repeated foils was lower after picture (mean=0.09, SD=0.1) than word encoding (mean=0.19, SD=0.18) (F=11.72, df=1, 68, p<0.001) in both groups. Despite schizophrenic patients’ higher level of false recognition of repeated foils, both schizophrenic and comparison subjects demonstrated a similar degree of false recognition suppression after picture encoding, compared to word encoding (interaction of encoding condition by group: F=1.45, df=1, 68, p=0.23) (Figure 2).

In addition to these significant main effects of group and encoding condition, there was also a significant effect of delay, such that longer delay to repetition (i.e., 24 words versus two words) led to a greater degree of false recognition (0.18 versus 0.10) (F=34.43, df=1, 68, p<0.0001). The effect of delay was more pronounced after word than picture encoding (interaction of delay and encoding condition: F=5.81, df=1, 68, p<0.05).

This accentuation of the effect of delay following word-encoding tended to be more pronounced in the comparison group (interaction of delay by encoding condition by group: F=3.26, df=1, 68, p=0.08), owing largely to a heightened degree of false recognition in the schizophrenic cohort at even the shortest delay interval (Figure 3). Indeed, the degree of false recognition after only a two-word delay was significantly higher in the schizophrenic group than in the comparison group (0.15 versus 0.05) (F=11.32, df=1, 68, p<0.005), particularly after word encoding (for two-word delay, interaction of encoding condition by group: F=4.09, df=1, 68, p<0.05).

We examined the potential confounding influence of mean parental education, encoding performance (syllable counting), and medication status on memory performance. Regression analyses indicated no significant association between mean parental education and either d′ (r=0.07, df=59, p=0.61) or false recognition (r=0.09, df=59, p=0.49). While syllable counting accuracy was slightly higher in the comparison subjects (mean=95.1%, SD=7.3%) than in the patients with schizophrenia (mean=89.3%, SD=12.1%) (t=2.38, df=70, p<0.05), syllable counting accuracy did not correlate with either d′ (r=0.14, df=71, p=0.24) or false recognition (r=0.05, df=71, p=0.71). There was no significant association between medication dose and either d′ (r=0.21, df=33, p=0.25) or false recognition (r=0.09, df=33, p=0.62) within the schizophrenic cohort. Patients taking atypical medication did not differ from those taking conventional medication in either d′ (t=0.03, df=37, p=0.98) or false recognition (t=0.12, df=37, p=0.91). Similarly, patients taking anticholinergic medication did not differ from other patients on either main measure of memory performance (d′: t=1.23, df=38, p=0.23; false recognition: t=0.89, df=38, p=0.38). Finally, given the potential for cognitive impairment even in first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia, we reran all analyses after excluding the single comparison subject with a positive family history. This did not affect the significance of any analyses.

Discussion

We found that patients with schizophrenia demonstrated an elevated rate of false recognition of repeated foils, relative to the comparison group. Despite this elevated rate of false recognition, patients with schizophrenia were just as successful in reducing false recognition after picture encoding (compared to word encoding). The former finding indicates a degraded ability to use item-specific recollection to sort old from new events in schizophrenia. The latter finding suggests an intact ability in schizophrenia to improve performance, even in the face of this deficit in conscious recollection.

These results add to a growing body of literature describing impaired verbal memory in patients with schizophrenia. These memory deficits include a decreased ability to identify the temporal and spatial context in which words were learned and an inability to assess the veridical nature of one’s own recollective experience, leading to elevated rates of false recognition (4, 10). This pattern of verbal memory deficits has been interpreted as evidence for a core cognitive deficit (impaired conscious awareness) and its neural substrate (impaired frontotemporal integration) in schizophrenia (10, 30–32). Our study provides further support for this type of specific cognitive deficit. We furthermore provide novel evidence that this deficit can be corrected by means of additional information at the time of learning. As verbal memory is a strong predictor of social functioning in schizophrenia (33), this latter finding may have important clinical implications. Remediation of cognitive impairment, an important approach to the long-term treatment of schizophrenic patients (3436), can rely on intact metacognitive strategies, even in the face of significant cognitive impairment.

Unlike other old/new recognition memory experiments, where reliance on item familiarity can be used to distinguish previously studied targets from novel foils, the paradigm used in this study penalized the use of such a strategy, as both targets and repeated foils are familiar. Correct discrimination of studied items from repeated foils therefore requires the use of a different mnemonic process, namely the recollection of source, or item-specific, information regarding the previous exposure to the stimulus (11, 12, 15, 37, 38). Ability to recall the temporal context in which the item had been previously experienced (i.e., during the study or test phase) allows for correct distinction between repeating foils and previously studied targets. The significantly greater degree of false recognition displayed by the schizophrenic group suggests an impairment in this recollective process. These results extend previous work demonstrating deficient source monitoring in schizophrenia, including evidence of poor temporal awareness (3942).

The apparent reliance on item familiarity, leading to the mistaken categorization of repeated foils as having been previously studied, also supports prior studies in schizophrenia that have found heightened reliance on familiarity-based memory processes (9, 10). Interestingly, this pattern of memory performance is similar to that of healthy elderly subjects, who also seem to be prone to basing mnemonic decisions on a feeling of familiarity, leading to a heightened degree of false recognition in this paradigm (11, 12, 15, 43), as well as in other tasks requiring old/new discrimination (4446). It is not clear which cognitive and neural mechanisms are responsible for the memory impairment in normal aging, but there is growing evidence that older persons utilize different functional brain networks, perhaps to compensate for reductions of efficiency in task-related brain areas (47, 48).

Patients with schizophrenia demonstrated elevated levels of false recognition to repeated novel stimuli after even a two-word delay, relative to the comparison subjects (Figure 3). This difficulty in properly categorizing an item in relation to one seen only a few moments earlier may represent a defect in verbal working memory processes in the schizophrenic group, a well-documented deficit in this population (4952). Unlike patients with schizophrenia, comparison subjects may be able to hold the previous representation of the foil “on-line” and therefore may be able to quickly and reliably categorize the repeating item as new.

Despite the elevated rate of false recognition to repeated foils, schizophrenic subjects were able to suppress the rate of false recognition after viewing pictures (as compared to words) in the study phase. As encoding condition (picture versus word) had no significant impact on old/new discriminability (as measured by d′), the finding of lower rates of false recognition of repeated items after picture encoding cannot be simply attributed to the greater ease of processing pictorial as compared to verbal information. This false recognition suppression after picture encoding, seen in prior studies of healthy adults, is ascribed to the use of a metacognitive strategy called the “distinctiveness heuristic” (15, 18, 19). Contrary to our initial hypothesis, it appears that this type of heuristic was operating in both the comparison and schizophrenic cohorts. This suggests that some mental heuristics may remain intact in schizophrenia, despite impairments in other metacognitive skills.

In line with prior studies of recognition memory in schizophrenia, this study found that patients demonstrated a slight impairment in overall recognition memory, as evidenced by a lower hit rate (correct recognition of previously studied items) and decreased discrimination accuracy (d′) (4, 9, 10). In contrast to patients in prior reports of response bias in schizophrenia, the patients in this study did not demonstrate an elevated false-alarm rate to the initial presentation of novel items (22, 23). This was in large part due to a significantly more conservative response strategy (i.e., greater hesitancy to call an item old) in the schizophrenic subjects than in the comparison subjects, a pattern similar to that recently demonstrated by Danion et al. (53). Given the discrepancies in the literature, the nature of response selection in schizophrenia, and its correlation with clinical features, may warrant further examination.

There are a number of limitations of this study that must be kept in mind while interpreting the results. First, all of the schizophrenic patients were taking psychotropic medications: differences in memory performance between these patients and the unmedicated healthy comparison group may be confounded by this factor. Prior work on the impact of medication status on episodic memory has been mixed, though most recent studies report no significant effect (54, 55) or even slight cognitive benefit (5658), particularly with atypical medications such as clozapine. Indeed, withdrawal of neuroleptic medication may even adversely affect memory performance (59). A lack of a significant correlation between neuroleptic dose and measures of memory performance within the schizophrenic cohort also argues against a significant impact of this variable.

Second, although the comparison subjects did not differ from the patients in age or gender, they had a higher overall level of attained education and a greater level of mean parental education. Level of achieved education is generally not considered an adequate measure for comparison of overall intellectual capability. The lack of a significant correlation between mean parental education and memory performance suggests that this variable is not critical to explaining the false recognition results. Furthermore, several findings, including the ability to suppress false recognition after picture encoding, were based on within-group comparisons where there were no significant differences in age, education, mean parental education, or medication status.

Finally, we studied an all-male cohort, a decision based on the predominance of male schizophrenic patients at our affiliated clinic. While this strategy has the advantage of providing a more homogeneous study group, it may decrease the generalizability of our results. There are no a priori reasons to expect different results in female subjects, nor have there been gender differences detected in prior experiments using this paradigm.

In conclusion, recognition memory is impaired in patients with schizophrenia, but this impairment can be overcome, at least in part, when additional information is provided at the time of learning. This finding is relevant for the rehabilitation of schizophrenic patients by means of cognitive behavior interventions.

TABLE 1
TABLE 2

Presented at the 5th Biennial Mt. Sinai Conference on Cognition in Schizophrenia, Whistler, British Columbia, Canada, April 27, 2001. Received Aug. 6, 2001; revisions received Dec. 27, 2001, and April 23, 2002; accepted April 25, 2002. From the Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital; Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, Mass.; and the Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, Charlottesville. Address reprint requests to Dr. Weiss, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Wang ACC 812, Boston, MA 02114; (e-mail). Supported by NIMH grant MH-01763 (Dr. Heckers), grant AG-08441 from the National Institute on Aging (Dr. Schacter), and a grant from the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression (Dr. Heckers).

Figure 1.

Figure 1. Summary of Study and Test Phases of the Memory Paradigm in a Study of Recognition Memory in Patients With Schizophrenia and Healthy Comparison Subjectsa

aIn the study phase, subjects saw either pictures or words presented concomitantly with an auditory stimulus. Subjects were instructed only to count the number of syllables in the word and were unaware of a subsequent memory test. (The images are not the original Snodgrass and Vanderwart [27] pictures used in the actual experiment.) In the test phase, items were presented in word form only, without an auditory stimulus. Previously studied target words were randomly intermixed with novel foils. The foils were then repeated, after either a two-word or 24-word delay. Subjects were asked to distinguish old words from new words, continuing to label the novel foils as new even when they repeated. (The color background is for illustration purposes only; all words were presented on a white background.)

Figure 2.

Figure 2. Rates of False Recognition of Repeated New Items Among Patients With Schizophrenia and Healthy Comparison Subjects Assigned to Picture or Word Encoding in a Study of Recognition Memorya

aFalse recognition was suppressed after picture encoding, compared with word encoding.

bCorrrected for rate of false recognition of new words at baseline.

cN=20 in word-encoding condition; N=20 in picture-encoding condition.

dN=16 in word-encoding condition; N=16 in picture-encoding condition.

Figure 3.

Figure 3. Rates of False Recognition of Repeated New Items After Repetition Delay Among Patients With Schizophrenia and Healthy Comparison Subjects Assigned to Picture or Word Encoding in a Study of Recognition Memory

aCorrected for rate of false recognition of new words at baseline.

bN=16 in word-encoding condition; N=16 in picture-encoding condition.

cN=20 in word-encoding condition; N=20 in picture-encoding condition.

References

1. Heaton RK, Paulsen JS, McAdams LA, Kuck J, Zisook S, Braff DL, Harris J, Jeste DV: Neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenics: relationship to age, chronicity, and dementia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994; 51:469-476Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2. Seidman LJ: Schizophrenia and brain dysfunction: an integration of recent neurodiagnostic findings. Psychol Bull 1983; 94:195-238Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3. Saykin AJ, Gur RC, Gur RE, Mozley PD, Mozley LH, Resnick SM, Kester DB, Stafiniak P: Neuropsychological function in schizophrenia: selective impairment in memory and learning. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48:618-624Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Aleman A, Hijman R, de Haan EHF, Kahn RS: Memory impairment in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:1358-1366AbstractGoogle Scholar

5. Clare L, McKenna PJ, Mortimer AM, Baddeley AD: Memory in schizophrenia: what is impaired and what is preserved? Neuropsychologia 1993; 31:1225-1241Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. Perry W, Light GA, Davis H, Braff DL: Schizophrenia patients demonstrate a dissociation on declarative and non-declarative memory tests. Schizophr Res 2000; 46:167-174Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Marie A, Gabrieli JD, Vaidya C, Brown B, Pratto F, Zajonc RB, Shaw RJ: The mere exposure effect in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 2001; 27:297-303Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8. Danion J-M, Meulemans T, Kauffmann-Muller F, Vermaat H: Intact implicit learning in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:944-948LinkGoogle Scholar

9. Huron C, Danion J-M, Giacomoni F, Grangé D, Robert P, Rizzo L: Impairment of recognition memory with, but not without, conscious recollection in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:1737-1742LinkGoogle Scholar

10. Danion JM, Rizzo L, Bruant A: Functional mechanisms underlying impaired recognition memory and conscious awareness in patients with schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:639-644Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11. Dodson CS, Schacter DL: When false recognition meets metacognition: the distinctiveness heuristic. J Memory and Language 2002; 46:782-803CrossrefGoogle Scholar

12. Jennings JM, Jacoby LL: An opposition procedure for detecting age-related deficits in recollection: telling effects of repetition. Psychol Aging 1997; 12:352-361Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Underwood BJ, Freund JS: Testing effects in the recognition of words. J Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 1970; 9:117-125CrossrefGoogle Scholar

14. Koriat A, Benzur H, Sheffer D: Telling the same story twice—output monitoring and age. J Memory and Language 1988; 27:23-39CrossrefGoogle Scholar

15. Schacter DL, Israel L, Racine C: Suppressing false recognition in younger and older adults: the distinctiveness heuristic. J Memory and Language 1999; 40:1-24CrossrefGoogle Scholar

16. Dywan J, Segalowitz SJ, Webster L: Source monitoring: ERP evidence for greater reactivity to nontarget information in older adults. Brain Cogn 1998; 36:390-430Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17. Israel L, Schacter DL: Pictorial encoding reduces false recognition of semantic associates. Psychon Bull Rev 1997; 4:577-581CrossrefGoogle Scholar

18. Schacter DL: Suppressing false memories: the distinctiveness heuristic and beyond. Int J Psychol 2000; 35:113-114Google Scholar

19. Dodson CS, Schacter DL: “If I had said it I would have remembered it”: reducing false memories with a distinctiveness heuristic. Psychon Bull Rev 2001; 8:155-161Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20. Smith TE, Hull JW, Israel LM, Willson DF: Insight, symptoms, and neurocognition in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. Schizophr Bull 2000; 26:193-200Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21. Lysaker PH, Bell MD, Bryson G, Kaplan E: Neurocognitive function and insight in schizophrenia: support for an association with impairments in executive function but not with impairments in global function. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1998; 97:297-301Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22. Brebion G, Smith MJ, Amador X, Malaspina D, Gorman JM: Word recognition, discrimination accuracy, and decision bias in schizophrenia: association with positive symptomatology and depressive symptomatology. J Nerv Ment Dis 1998; 186:604-609Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23. Brebion G, Smith MJ, Gorman JM, Amador X: Discrimination accuracy and decision biases in different types of reality monitoring in schizophrenia. J Nerv Ment Dis 1997; 185:247-253Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders, Patient Edition (SCID-P), version 2. New York, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research, 1995Google Scholar

25. Kay SR, Opler LA, Fishbein A: Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) Rating Manual. Toronto, Multi Health Systems, 1992Google Scholar

26. Fischler I, Juola JF: Effects of repeated tests on recognition time for information in long-term memory. J Exp Psychol 1971; 91:54-58CrossrefGoogle Scholar

27. Snodgrass JG, Vanderwart M: A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. J Exp Psychol Hum Learn 1980; 6:174-215Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28. Macmillan NA, Creelman CD: Detection Theory: A User’s Guide. New York, Cambridge University Press, 1991Google Scholar

29. Snodgrass JG, Corwin J: Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: applications to dementia and amnesia. J Exp Psychol Gen 1988; 117:34-50Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30. Dolan RJ, Fletcher PC, McKenna PJ, Friston K, Frith CD: Abnormal neural integration related to cognition in schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl 1999; 99:58-67CrossrefGoogle Scholar

31. Frith C: When conscious recollection disrupts memory. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:645-646Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32. Heckers S, Rauch SL, Goff D, Savage CR, Schacter DL, Fischman AJ, Alpert NM: Impaired recruitment of the hippocampus during conscious recollection in schizophrenia. Nat Neurosci 1998; 1:318-323Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33. Green MF: What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? Am J Psychiatry 1996; 153:321-330LinkGoogle Scholar

34. Bellack AS, Gold JM, Buchanan RW: Cognitive rehabilitation for schizophrenia: problems, prospects, and strategies. Schizophr Bull 1999; 25:257-274Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35. Green MF, Nuechterlein KH: Should schizophrenia be treated as a neurocognitive disorder? Schizophr Bull 1999; 25:309-319Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36. Spaulding WD, Reed D, Sullivan M, Richardson C, Weiler M: Effects of cognitive treatment in psychiatric rehabilitation. Schizophr Bull 1999; 25:657-676Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37. Johnson MK: Source monitoring and memory distortion. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1997; 352:1733-1745Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38. Johnson MK, Hashtroudi S, Lindsay DS: Source monitoring. Psychol Bull 1993; 114:3-28Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39. Keefe RSE, Arnold MC, Bayen UJ, Harvey PD: Source monitoring deficits in patients with schizophrenia: a multinomial modelling analysis. Psychol Med 1999; 29:903-914Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

40. Vinogradov S, Willis-Shore J, Poole JH, Marten E, Ober BA, Shenaut GK: Clinical and neurocognitive aspects of source monitoring errors in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154:1530-1537LinkGoogle Scholar

41. Keefe RSE, Courtney M, McEvoy JM: Self-monitoring deficits in schizophrenia: autonoetic agnosia (abstract). Schizophr Res 1997; 24:110CrossrefGoogle Scholar

42. Elvevag B, Egan MF, Goldberg TE: Memory for temporal order in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2000; 46:187-193Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

43. Jennings JM, Jacoby LL: Improving age-related deficits in recollection: application of an opposition procedure. Brain Cogn 1997; 35:403-406Google Scholar

44. Norman KA, Schacter DL: False recognition in younger and older adults: exploring the characteristics of illusory memories. Mem Cogn 1997; 25:838-848Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

45. Koutstaal W, Schacter DL: Gist-based false recognition of pictures in older and younger adults. J Memory and Language 1997; 37:555-583CrossrefGoogle Scholar

46. Kensinger EA, Schacter DL: When true memories suppress false memories: effects of ageing. Cognitive Neuropsychology 1999; 16:399-415CrossrefGoogle Scholar

47. Grady CL: Functional brain imaging and age-related changes in cognition. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 54:259-281CrossrefGoogle Scholar

48. Grady CL, Craik FE: Changes in memory processing with age. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2000; 10:224-231Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

49. Bell MD, Bryson G, Wexler B: Verbal working memory impairment in schizophrenia (letter). Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:660LinkGoogle Scholar

50. Elvevag B, Weinberger DR, Goldberg TE: Short-term memory for serial order in schizophrenia: a detailed examination of error types. Neuropsychology 2001; 15:128-135Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

51. Conklin HM, Curtis CE, Katsanis J, Iacono WG: Verbal working memory impairment in schizophrenia patients and their first-degree relatives: evidence from the Digit Span Task. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:275-277LinkGoogle Scholar

52. Stone M, Gabrieli JD, Stebbins GT, Sullivan EV: Working and strategic memory deficits in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology 1998; 12:278-288Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

53. Danion J-M, Gokalsing E, Robert P, Massin-Krauss M, Bacon E: Defective relationship between subjective experience and behavior in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:2064-2066LinkGoogle Scholar

54. Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR: Effects of neuroleptic medications on the cognition of patients with schizophrenia: a review of recent studies. J Clin Psychiatry 1996; 57(suppl 9):62-65Google Scholar

55. Mortimer AM: Cognitive function in schizophrenia—do neuroleptics make a difference? Pharm Biochem Behav 1997; 56:789-795Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

56. Hagger C, Buckley P, Kenny JT, Friedman L, Ubogy D, Meltzer HY: Improvement in cognitive functions and psychiatric symptoms in treatment-refractory schizophrenic patients receiving clozapine. Biol Psychiatry 1993; 34:702-712Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

57. Keefe RS, Bollini AM, Silva SG: Do novel antipsychotics improve cognition? a report of a meta-analysis. Psychiatr Annals 1999; 29:623-629CrossrefGoogle Scholar

58. Stip E: Memory impairment in schizophrenia: perspectives from psychopathology and pharmacotherapy. Can J Psychiatry 1996; 41:S27-S34Google Scholar

59. Gilbertson MW, van Kammen DP: Recent and remote memory dissociation: medication effects and hippocampal function in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 1997; 42:585-595Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar