Nervenheilkunde 2012; 31(03): 180-184
DOI: 10.1055/s-0038-1628279
Geist & Gehirn
Schattauer GmbH

Imitieren und/oder Improvisieren(?)

Gemeinsam mehr Freude und Effektivität
M. Spitzer
Further Information

Publication History





Publication Date:
23 January 2018 (online)

 

 
  • Literatur

  • 1 Christakis DA, Zimmerman FJ, Garrison MM. Effect of block play on language acquisition and attention in toddlers. A pilot randomized controlled trial. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2007; 161: 967-71.
  • 2 Clark, Krych.. Speaking while monitoring addressees for understanding. J Mem Lang 2004; 50: 62-81.
  • 3 Gergely G, Bekkering H, Király I. Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature 2002; 415: 755.
  • 4 Goldin-Meadow S. et al. Gesturing gives children new ideas about math. Psychological Science 2009; 20: 267-72.
  • 5 Johnstone K. Impro. Improvisation and the theatre. Methuen Drama. London, UK: A & C Black Publishers; 1979
  • 6 Kiefer M. et al. Experience-dependent plasticity of conceptual representations in human sensory-motor areas. J Cognitive Neuroscience 2007; 19: 525-42.
  • 7 Limb CJ, Brown AR. Neural substrates of spontaneous musical performance: an FMRI study of jazz improvisation. PloS ONE 2008; 3 (Suppl. 02) e1679.
  • 8 Marsh KL. et al. Hop off the mirror neuron bandwagon and join ours, it’s less crowded!. European Journal of Social Psychology 2009; 39: 1234-5.
  • 9 Meltzoff AN, Moore MK. Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 1977; 198: 75-8.
  • 10 Meltzoff AN. Foundations for developing a concept of self: the role of imitation in relating self to other and the value of social mirroring, social modeling, and self practice in infancy. In: Cicchetti D, Beeghly M. (eds.) The self in transition: infancy to childhood. Chicago, IL: University Press; 1990
  • 11 Meltzoff AN, Decety J. What imitation tells us about social cognition: a rapprochement between developmental psychologyand cognitive neuroscience. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B 2003; 358: 491-500.
  • 12 Noy L, Dekel E, Alon U. The mirror game as a paradigm for studying the dynamics oft wo people improvising motion together. PNAS 2011; 108: 20947-52.
  • 13 Oullier O. et al. Social coordination dynamics: Measuring human bonding. Soc Neurosci 2008; 3: 178-92.
  • 14 Plotnik JM, de Waal F, Reiss D. Self-recognition in an Asian elephant. PNAS 2006; 103: 17053-7.
  • 15 Prior H, Schwarz A, Güntürkün O. Mirror-induced behavior in the magpie (Pica pica): Evidence of Self-Recognition. PLoS Biol 2008; 6 (Suppl. 08) e202.
  • 16 Richardson DC, Dale R. Looking to understand: The coupling between speakers’ and listeners’ eye movements and its relationship to discourse comprehension. Cognitive Science 2005; 29: 39-54.
  • 17 Richardson MJ, Marsh KL, Schmidt RC. Effects of visual and verbal interaction on unintentional interpersonal coordination. J Experimental Psychology 2005; 31: 62-79.
  • 18 Richardson MJ. et al. Rocking together: Dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. Human Movement Science 2007; 26: 867-91.
  • 19 Rowe ML, Goldin-Meadow S. Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in child vocabulary size at school entry. Science 2009; 323: 951-3.
  • 20 Sebanz N, Bekkering H, Knolich G. Joint action: bodies and minds moving together. Trends in Cognitive Science 2006; 10: 70-6.
  • 21 Sebanz N, Knoblich G. Jumping on the ecological bandwagon? Mind the gap!. European Journal of Social Psychology 2009; 39: 1230-3.
  • 22 Stephens GJ, Silbert LJ, Hasson U. Speaker-listener neural coupling underlies successful communication. PNAS USA 2010; 107: 14425-30.
  • 23 Vallacher RR, Jackson D. Thinking inside the boxdynamical constraints on mind and action: Comment on Marsh et al.’s “Toward a radically embodied, embedded social psychology”. European Journal of Social Psychology 2009; 39: 1226-9.
  • 24 Andrew N. et al. Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science. 1977 198(4312).
  • 25 Spitzer M. Musik im Kopf. Stuttgart: Schattauer; 2002: 347-58.