Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1026//0033-3042.51.3.135

Zusammenfassung. Aus der Perspektive des Zweikomponentenmodells der Kognitionsontogenese, das der biologischen Mechanik die kulturell geformte Pragmatik gegenüberstellt, wird die intellektuelle Entwicklung über die Lebensspanne nachgezeichnet. Besonders berücksichtigt werden folgende Gegenstände: (a) Formen der Abhängigkeit zwischen mechanischer und pragmatischer Entwicklung; (b) ontogenetische Veränderungen in relativer Stabilität, Heritabilität und Differenzierung intellektueller Fähigkeiten; (c) historische und ontogenetische Plastizität des Leistungsniveaus; (d) die Purifizierung der Messung von Altersunterschieden in der Mechanik; und (e) die Suche nach basalen Determinanten der mechanischen Entwicklung. Plädiert wird für eine Konzeptualisierung intellektueller Entwicklung, die bereichsspezifische und übergreifende Aspekte gleichermaßen berücksichtigt.


Intellectual development across the lifespan: Overview and research perspectives

Abstract. Informed by the distinction between biology-based mechanics and cultural pragmatics, this article summarizes intellectual development from a lifespan perspective. Topics include: (a) varieties of mechanic-pragmatic interdependence; (b) ontogenetic changes in relative stability, heritability, and factor structure differentiation; (c) historical and ontogenetic plasticity of intellectual performance; (d) age differences in the mechanics of cognition: purification of measurement; and (e) the search for basic determinants of age changes in the mechanics of cognition. It is argued that a valid conceptualization of intellectual development needs to do justice to both domain-general and domain-specific aspects of change.

Literatur

  • Baddeley, A. (1996). Exploring the central executive.. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 5– 28 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baltes, M. M. Carstensen, L. L. (1996). The processes of successful ageing.. Ageing and Society, 16, 397– 422 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baltes, P. B. (1968). Longitudinal and cross-sectional sequences in the study of age and generation effects.. Human Development, 11, 145– 171 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baltes, P. B. (1997). Die unvollendete Architektur der menschlichen Ontogenese: Implikationen für die Zukunft des vierten Lebensalters.. Psychologische Rundschau, 48, 191– 210 Google Scholar

  • Baltes, P. B. Kliegl, R. (1992). Further testing of limits of cognitive plasticity: Negative age differences in a mnemonic skill are robust.. Developmental Psychology, 28, 121– 125 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baltes, P. B. Lindenberger, U. (1997). Emergence of a powerful connection between sensory and cognitive functions across the adult life span: A new window to the study of cognitive aging?. Psychology and Aging, 12, 12– 21 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baltes, P. B. Lindenberger, U. Staudinger, U. M. (1998). Life-span theory in developmental psychology.. In W. Damon (Eds.) & R. M. Lerner (Bd. Hrsg.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5. Aufl., pp. 1029-1143). New York: Wiley. Google Scholar

  • Baltes, P. B. Staudinger, U. M. Lindenberger, U. (1999). Lifespan psychology: Theory and application to intellectual functioning.. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 471– 507 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bosman, E. A. Charness, N. (1996). Age-related differences in skilled performance and skill acquisition.. In F. Blanchard-Fields & T. M. Hess (Eds.), Perspectives on cognitive change in adulthood and aging (pp. 428-453). New York: McGraw-Hill. Google Scholar

  • Cardon, L. R. Fulker, D. W. (1994). A model of developmental change in hierarchical phenotypes with application to specific cognitive abilities.. Behavior Genetics, 24, 1– 16 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: Their structure, growth, and action. . Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin. Google Scholar

  • Cerella, J. (1990). Aging and information-processing rate.. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (3. Aufl., pp. 201-221). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Google Scholar

  • Deary, I. J. Egan, V. Gibson, G. J. Austin, E. J. Brand, C. Kellaghan, T. (1996). Intelligence and the differentiation hypothesis.. Intelligence, 23, 105– 132 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Erdfelder, E. Rietz, C. Rudinger, G. (1996). Methoden der Entwicklungspsychologie.. In E. Erdfelder, R. Mausfeld, T. Meiser & G. Rudinger (Eds.), Handbuch Quantitative Methoden (pp. 539-550). Weinheim: Psychologie Verlags Union. Google Scholar

  • Ericsson, K. A. Lehmann, A. C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence of maximal adaption to task constraints.. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 273– 305 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fischer, K. W. Rose, S. P. (1994). Dynamic development of coordination of components in brain and behavior: A framework for theory and research.. In G. Dawson & K. W. Fischer (Hrsg.), Human behavior and the developing brain (S. 3-66). New York: Guilford. Google Scholar

  • Fry, A. F. Hale, S. (1996). Processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelligence: Evidence for a developmental cascade.. Psychological Science, 7, 237– 241 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Guthke, J. Wiedl, K. H. Hrsg. (1996). Dynamisches Testen. . Göttingen: Hogrefe. Google Scholar

  • Gobet, F. Simon, H. A. (1996). The roles of recognition processes and look-ahead search in time-constrained expert problem solving: Evidence from grand-master-level chess.. Psychological Science, 7, 52– 55 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hasher, L. Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view.. In G. H. Bower (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Bd. 22, pp. 193-225). Google Scholar

  • Hertzog, C. Schaie, K. W. (1986). Stability and change in adult intelligence: 1. Analysis of longitudinal covariance structures.. Psychology and Aging, 1, 159– 171 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Horn, J. L. (1989). Models of intelligence.. In R. L. Linn (Eds.), Intelligence: Measurement, theory, and public policy (pp. 29-73). Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press. Google Scholar

  • Humphreys, L. G. Davey, T. C. (1988). Continuity in intellectual growth from 12 months to 9 years.. Intelligence, 12, 183– 197 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Just, M. A. Carpenter, P. A. Keller, T.A. (1996). The capacity theory of comprehension: New frontiers of evidence and arguments.. Psychological Review, 103, 773– 780 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Klauer, K. J. (1994). Über den Einfluß eines Trainings zum induktiven Denken auf Variablen der fluiden Intelligenz und des Lernens bei älteren Menschen.. Zeitschrift für Gerontopsychologie und -psychiatrie, 7, 29– 46 Google Scholar

  • Kliegl, R. Mayr, U. Krampe, R. T. (1994). Time-accuracy functions for determining process and person differences: An application to cognitive aging.. Cognitive Psychology, 26, 134– 164 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Li, S.-C. Lindenberger, U. (1999). Cross-level unification: A computational exploration of the link between deterioration of neurotransmitter systems and dedifferentiation of cognitive abilities in old age.. In L.-G. Nilsson & H. J. Markowitsch (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of memory (pp. 103-146). Google Scholar

  • Lindenberger, U. (in Druck-a). Kognitive Intervention im Alter: Anwendungsorientierte Interpretation zentraler Befunde.. In H.-W. Wahl & C. Tesch-Römer (Hrsg.), Angewandte Gerontologie in Schlüsselbegriffen. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer. Google Scholar

  • Lindenberger, U. (in Druck-b). Avec l'âge, il existe un lien entre fonctionnement sensoriel ou sensorimoteur et fonctionnement cognitif.. In Le vieillissement normal: Approche cognitive. Belgium: DeBoek. Google Scholar

  • Lindenberger, U. Baltes, P. B. (1994). Sensory functioning and intelligence in old age: A powerful connection.. Psychology and Aging, 9, 339– 355 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lindenberger, U. Baltes, P. B. (1995). Testing-the-limits and experimental simulation: Two methods to explicate the role of learning in development.. Human Development, 38, 349– 360 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lindenberger, U. Baltes, P. B. (1997). Intellectual functioning in old and very old age: Cross-sectional results from the Berlin Aging Study.. Psychology and Aging, 12, 410– 432 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lindenberger, U. Baltes, P. B. (1999). Die Entwicklungspsychologie der Lebensspanne (Lifespan-Psychologie): Johann Nicolaus Tetens (1736-1807) zu Ehren.. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 207, 299– 323 Google Scholar

  • Lindenberger, U. Baltes, P. B. (in Druck). Lifespan psychology: Theory.. In A. E. Kazdin (Ed.), Encyclopedia of psychology. Washington: APA and Oxford University Press. Google Scholar

  • Lindenberger, U. Mayr, U. Kliegl, R. (1993). Speed and intelligence in old age.. Psychology and Aging, 8, 207– 220 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lindenberger, U. Pötter, U. (1998). The complex nature of unique and shared effects in hierarchical linear regression: Implications for developmental psychology.. Psychological Methods, 3, 218– 230 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Marshalek, B. Lohman, D. F. Snow, R. E. (1983). The complexity continuum in the radex and hierarchical models of intelligence.. Intelligence, 7, 107– 127 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mayr, U. Kliegl, R. Krampe, R. T. (1996). Sequential and coordinative processing dynamics in figural transformations across the life span.. Cognition, 59, 61– 90 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McCall, R. M. Carriger, M. S. (1993). A meta-analysis of infant habituation and recognition memory performance as predictors of later IQ.. Child Development, 64, 57– 79 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McClearn, G. E. Johansson, B. Berg, S. Pedersen, N. L. Ahern, F. Petrill, S. A. Plomin, R. (1997). Substantial genetic influence on cognitive abilities in twins 80 or more years old.. Science, 276, 1560– 1563 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McGue, M. Bouchard, T. J., Jr. Iacono, W. G. Lykken, D. T. (1993). Behavioral genetics of cognitive ability: A life-span perspective.. In R. Plomin & G. E. McClearn (Eds.), Nature, nurture, and psychology (pp. 59-76). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Google Scholar

  • Molenaar, P. C. M. Boomsma, D. I. Dolan, C. V. (1991). Genetic and environmental factors in a developmental perspective.. In D. Magnusson, L. Bergman, G. Rudinger & B. Törestad (Eds.), Problems and methods in longitudinal research: Stability and change (pp. 250-273). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Google Scholar

  • Piaget, J. (1977). Recherches sur l'abstraction réfléchissante. 1: L'abstraction des relations logico-arithmétiques. . Paris: Presses Universitaires de France. Google Scholar

  • Salthouse, T. A. (1991). Theoretical perspectives on cognitive aging. . Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Google Scholar

  • Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition.. Psychological Review, 103, 403– 428 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Scarr, S. McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environment: A theory of genotype environment effects.. Child Development, 54, 424– 435 Google Scholar

  • Schaie, K. W. (1996). Adult intellectual development: The Seattle Longitudinal Study. . New York: Cambridge University Press. Google Scholar

  • Siegler, R. S. Crowley, K. (1994). Constraints on learning in nonprivileged domains.. Cognitive Psychology, 27, 194– 226 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spearman, C. E. (1927). The abilities of man. . London: Macmillan. Google Scholar

  • Staudinger, U. M. Pasupathi, M. (in Druck). Life-span perspectives on self, personality, and social cognition.. In T. Salthouse & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and aging. Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Google Scholar

  • Verhaeghen, P. Salthouse, T. A. (1997). Meta-analyses of age-cognition relations in adulthood: Estimates of linear and non-linear age effects and structural models.. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 231– 249 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Weinert, F. E. Perner, J. (1996). Cognitive development.. In D. Magnusson (Eds.), The life-span development of individuals: Behavioural, neurobiological, and psychosocial perspectives. A synthesis. (S. 207-222). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Google Scholar

  • Wellman, H. M. Gelman, S. A. (1992). Cognitive development: Foundational theories of core domains.. Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 337– 375 CrossrefGoogle Scholar