Skip to main content
Log in

Performance and Private Speech of Children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder While Taking the Tower of Hanoi Test: Effects of Depth of Search, Diagnostic Subtype, and Methylphenidate

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We administered the Tower of Hanoi to demographically comparable samples of control participants (n = 34) and children with the Combined (n = 22) and Inattentive subtypes (n = 19) of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Controls excelled over children with the Inattentive subtype, who outperformed patients with the Combined subtype. These results replicated findings of greater executive deficits in the Combined than in the Inattentive type of ADHD. Double-blind administration of methylphenidate improved task performance only for patients with the Inattentive subtype. In a drug-free Baseline session, children with both subtypes of ADHD made more private verbalizations than controls, particularly when failing puzzles. In later sessions, regardless of drug condition, the inattentive sample exhibited a smaller increase in self-regulatory utterances under failure. In contrast, the combined sample decreased self-regulatory verbalizations under failure only under methylphenidate. The results support some differences between the two subtypes of ADHD in executive functioning and in their response to stimulant therapy.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ackerman, R. T., Anhalt, J. M., Dykman, R. A., & Holcomb, P. J. (1986). Effortful processing deficits in children with reading and/or attention disorders. Brain and Cognition, 5, 22–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aman, C. J., Roberts, R. J., Jr., & Pennington, B. F. (1998). A neuropsychological examination of the underlying deficit in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Frontal lobe versus right parietal lobe theories. Developmental Psychology, 34, 956–969.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A. (1997). ADHD and the nature of self-control. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A., DuPaul, G. J., & McMurray, B. (1990). Comprehensive evaluation of attention deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity as defined by research criteria. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, 775–789.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A., DuPaul, G. J., & McMurray, B. (1991). Attention Deficit Disorder with and without hyperactivity: Clinical response to three dose levels of methylphenidate. Pediatrics, 87, 519–531.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A., Grodzinsky, G., & DuPaul, G. J. (1992). Frontal lobe functions in Attention Deficit Disorder with and without hyperactivity: A review and research report. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 20, 163–188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beaudichon, J. (1973). Nature and instrumental function of private speech in problem solving situations. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 19, 117–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berk, L. E. (1986). Relationship of elementary school children's private speech to behavioral accompaniment to task, attention, and task performance. Developmental Psychology, 22, 671–680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berk, L. E., & Garvin, R. A. (1984). Development of private speech among low-income Appalachian children. Developmental Psychology, 20, 271–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berk, L. E., & Landau, S. (1993). Private speech of learning disabled and normally achieving children in classroom academic and laboratory contexts. Child Development, 64, 556–571.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berk, L. E., & Potts, M. (1991). Development and functional significance of private speech among attention-deficit hyperactivity disordered and normal boys. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 19, 357–377.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M., Aamodt-Leeper, G., Creswell, C., McGurk, R., & Skuse, D. H. (2001). Individual differences in cognitive planning on the Tower of Hanoi task: Neuropsychological maturity or measurement error? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 551–556.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bivens, J. A., & Berk, L. E. (1990). A longitudinal study of the development of elementary school children's private speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 36, 443–463.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borys, S. V., Spitz, H. H., & Dorans, B. A. (1982). Tower of Hanoi performance of retarded young adults and nonretarded children as a function of solution length and goal state. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33, 87–110.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, S. B. (1973). Cognitive styles in reflective, impulsive, and hyperactive boys and their mothers. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 36, 747–752.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, C. L., & Mann, M. (2000). Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, predominantly inattentive subtype. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 9, 499–510.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chhabildas, N. Pennington, B. F., & Willcutt, E. G. (2001). A comparison of the neuropsychological profiles of the DSM-IV subtypes of ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 529–540.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chang, H. T. (2000). Effects of methylphenidate on performance and private speech of children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder during the Tower of Hanoi Task. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY.

  • Copeland, A. P. (1979). Types of private speech produced by hyperactive and nonhyperactive boys. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 7, 169–177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Culbertson, W. C., & Zillmer, E. A. (2001). Tower of London. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems.

    Google Scholar 

  • Douglas, V. I. (1999). Cognitive control processes in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. In H. C. Quay & A. E. Hogan (Eds.), Handbook of disruptive behavior disorders (pp. 105–138). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • DuPaul, G. J. (1991). Parent and teacher ratings of ADHD symptoms: Psychometric properties in a community-based sample. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 20, 242–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., Weber, W., & Russell, R. L. (1998). Psychiatric, neuropsychological, and psychosocial features of DSM-IV subtypes of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Results from a clinically referred sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 37, 185–193.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frauenglass, M., & Diaz, R. (1985). Self-regulatory functions of children's private speech: A critical analysis of recent challenges to Vygotsky's theory. Developmental Psychology, 21, 357–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goel, V., & Grafman, J. (1995). Are the frontal lobes implicated in planning functions? Interpreting data from the Tower of Hanoi. Neuropsychologia, 5, 623–642.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, S. H. (1981). The integration of verbal and motor behavior in preschool children. Child Development, 52, 280–289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinshaw, S. P. (2001). Is the inattentive type of ADHD a separate disorder? Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 8, 498–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Houghton, S., Douglas, G., West, J., Whiting, K., Wall, M., Langsford, S., Powell, L., & Carroll, A. (1999). Differential patterns of executive function in children with Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder according to gender and subtype. Journal of Child Neurology, 14, 801–805.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. C. (1985). Kaufman tests of academic achievement. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Brent, D., Rao, U., Flynn, C., Moreci, P., Williamson, D., & Ryan, N. (1998). Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children—Present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. Journal of American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 980–988.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, W. L. (1964). An investigation of the spontaneous speech of children during problem solving. Dissertation Abstracts International, 25, 2031. (University Microfilms No. 64–09, 240).

  • Klorman, R., Hazel-Fernandez, L. A., Shaywitz, S. L., Fletcher, J. M., Marchione, K. E., Holahan, J. M., Stuebing, K. K., Brumaghim, J. T., & Shaywitz, B. A. (1999). Executive functioning deficits in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder are independent of oppositional defiant or reading disorder. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 1148–1155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. Yaeger, J., & Hjertholm, E. (1968). Private speech: Four studies and a review of theories. Child Development, 39, 691–736.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lahey, B., & Carlson, C. (1991). Validity of diagnostic category of attention deficit disorder without hyperactivity: A review of the literature. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 24, 110–120.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McBurnett, K. (2000). Methylphenidate treatment of DSM-IV types of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. In L. Greenhill, and B. Osman (Eds.), Ritalin Theory and practice (2nd ed., pp. 253–263). Larchmont, NY: M. A. Liebert.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meichenbaum, D., & Biemiller, A. (1990, May). In search of student expertise in the classroom: A metacognitive analysis. Paper presented at the conference on Cognitive Research for Instructional Innovation, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland.

  • Meichenbaum, D., & Goodman, S. (1979). Clinical use of private speech and critical questions about its study in natural settings. In G. Zivin (Ed.), The development of self-regulation through private speech (pp. 325–360). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milich, R., Balentine, A. C., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). ADHD combined type and ADHD predominantly inattentive type are distinct and unrelated disorders. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 8, 463–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MTA Cooperative Group (1999). A 14-month randomized clinical trial of treatment strategies for Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 1073–1086.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, J. D. (1979). Spontaneous private speech and performance on a delayed match-to-sample task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 27, 286–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nigg, J. T., Blaskey, L. G., Huang-Pollock, C., & Rappley, M. D. (2002). Neuropsychological executive functions and DSM-IV ADHD subtypes. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 59–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Owen, A. M., Downes, J. J., Sahakian, B. J., Polkey, C. E., & Robbins, T. W. (1990). Planning and spatial working memory following frontal lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia, 28, 1021–1034.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., & Jensen, J. (1999). Specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 171–177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paternite, C. E., Loney, J., & Roberts, M. A. (1996). A preliminary validation of subtypes of DSM-IV Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of Attention Disorders, 1, 70–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., Groisser, D., & Welsh, M. C. (1993). Contrasting cognitive deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder versus reading disability. Developmental Psychology, 29, 511–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 51–87.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schachar, R., Ickowicz, A., & Wachsmuth, R. (1995). Parent Interview for Child Symptoms (P.I.C.S.—4). Unpublished document, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, ON Canada.

  • Sergeant, J. A., Geurts, H., & Oosterlaan, J. (2002). How specific is a deficit of executive functioning for Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder? Behavioural Brain Research, 130, 3–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 298, 199–209.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. (1975). The functional equivalence of problem solving skills. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 268–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spitz, H. H., Minsky, S. K., & Bessellieu, C. L. (1984). Subgoal length versus full solution length in predicting Tower of Hanoi problem-solving performance. Bulletin of Psychonomic Society, 22, 301–304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spitz, H. H., Minsky, S. K., & Bessellieu, C. L. (1985). Influence of planning time and first-move strategy on Tower of Hanoi problem-solving performance of mentally retarded young adults and nonretarded children. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 90, 49–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spitz, H. H., Webster, N. A., & Borys, S. V. (1982). Further studies of the Tower of Hanoi problem-solving performance of retarded young adults and nonretarded children. Developmental Psychology, 18, 922–930.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. (1934/1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1991). Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Third edition. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weyandt, L. L. & Willis, W. G. (1994). Executive functions in school-aged children: Potential efficacy of tasks in discriminating clinical groups. Developmental Neuropsychology, 10, 27–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winsler, A. (1998). Parent-child interactions and private speech in boys with ADHD. Applied Developmental Science, 2, 17–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W., & Johnson, M. B. (1989). Woodcock-Johnson Psychoeducational Battery—Revised. Allen, TX: DLM Teaching Resources.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zentall, S. S., Gohs, D. E., & Culatta, B. (1983). Language and activity of hyperactive and comparison children during listening tasks. Exceptional Children, 50, 255–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zivin, G. (1972). Functions of private speech during problem-solving in preschool children. Dissertation Abstracts International, 33(2-B), 1834. (University Microfilms No. 72–26, 224).

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rafael Klorman.

Additional information

Portions of these results were submitted as part of H. Theresa Chang's PhD dissertation (Chang, 2000) and Helena Kopecky's unpublished master's research, both conducted at the University of Rochester and supervised by Rafael Klorman.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kopecky, H., Chang, H.T., Klorman, R. et al. Performance and Private Speech of Children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder While Taking the Tower of Hanoi Test: Effects of Depth of Search, Diagnostic Subtype, and Methylphenidate. J Abnorm Child Psychol 33, 625–638 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-005-6742-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-005-6742-7

Key Words

Navigation