Skip to main content

Facilitating Effective Mental Processes: The Imagination and Self-Explanation Effects

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Cognitive Load Theory

Abstract

According to the expertise reversal effect (Chapter 12) during the initial phases of skill acquisition, worked examples (Chapter 8) represent an efficient form of instruction, while problem-solving practice is superior during later phases of skill acquisition. This reversal suggests that as levels of expertise increase, levels of instructional guidance should decrease. Completion tasks (Chapter 8) and faded worked examples (Chapter 13) were suggested as instructional procedures that can be used to gradually decrease the levels of guidance. Empirical evidence has indicated that these methods are a more effective and efficient means for a smooth transition from initial instruction based on worked examples to later problem-solving practice. This chapter reviews evidence supporting an alternative to studying worked examples or problem-solving practice for more knowledgeable learners. The method is based on imagining activities, procedures, or concepts, for example, imagining a problem solution provided in a recently ­studied worked example. Imagining is defined as the mental reproduction of a procedure or a concept.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 149.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Bielaczyc, K., Pirolli, P. L., & Brown, A. L. (1995). Training in self-explanation and self-regulation strategies: Investigating the effects of knowledge acquisition activities on problem solving. Cognition and Instruction, 13, 221–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T., Bassok, M., Lewis, M. W., Reimann, P., & Glaser, R. (1989). Self-explanations: How students study and use examples in learning to solve problems. Cognitive Science, 13, 145–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H., de Leeuw, N., Chiu, M.-H., & LaVancher, C. (1994). Eliciting self-explanations improves understanding. Cognitive Science, 18, 439–477.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, L. V. (1960). Effect of mental practice on the development of a certain motor skill. Research Quarterly of the American Association for Health, Physical Education, and Recreation, 31, 560–569.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, R., Nguyen, F., & Sweller, J. (2006). Efficiency in learning: Evidence-based guidelines to manage cognitive load. San Francisco: Pfeiffer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, G., Tindall-Ford, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2001). Learning by imagining. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7, 68–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corbin, C. B. (1967). The effects of covert rehearsal on the development of a complex motor skill. The Journal of General Psychology, 76, 143–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Driskell, J. E., Copper, C., & Moran, A. (1994). Does mental practice enhance performance? The Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 481–492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (2000). Causal reasoning, mental rehearsal, and the evolution of primate cognition. In C. Heyes & L. Luber (Eds.), The evolution of cognition: Vienna series in theoretical biology (pp. 205–219). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Egstrom, G. H. (1964). Effects of an emphasis on conceptualizing techniques during early learning of a gross motor skill. Research Quarterly, 35, 472–481.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance: Its structure and acquisition. The American Psychologist, 49, 725–747.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Etnier, J. L., & Landers, D. M. (1996). The influence of procedural variables on the efficacy of mental practice. The Sport Psychologist, 10, 48–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ginns, P. (2005a). Imagining instructions: Mental practice in highly cognitive domains. Australian Journal of Education, 49, 128–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ginns, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). When imagining information is effective. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28, 229–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grouios, G. (1992). The effect of mental practice on diving performance. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 23, 60–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hazler, R. J., & Hipple, T. E. (1981). The effects of mental practice on counselling behaviours. Counselor Education and Supervision, 20, 211–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelsey, I. B. (1961). Effects of mental practice and physical practice upon muscular endurance. Research Quarterly of the American Association for Health, Physical Education, and Recreation, 32, 47–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leahy, W., & Sweller, J. (2004). Cognitive load and the imagination effect. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 857–875.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leahy, W., & Sweller, J. (2005). Interactions among the imagination, expertise reversal, and element interactivity effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 11, 266–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leahy, W., & Sweller, J. (2008). The imagination effect increases with an increased intrinsic cognitive load. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 273–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leutner, D., Leopold, C., & Sumfleth, E. (2009). Cognitive load and science text comprehension: Effects of drawing and mentally imagining text content. Computers in Human Behavior, 25, 284–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mendoza, D., & Wichman, H. (1978). ‘Inner’ darts: Effects of mental practice on performance of dart throwing. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47, 1195–1199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mwangi, W., & Sweller, J. (1998). Learning to solve compare word problems: The effect of example format and generating self-explanations. Cognition and Instruction, 16, 173–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perry, H. M. (1939). The relative efficiency of actual and ‘imaginary’ practice in five selected tasks. Archives of Psychology, 243, 76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phipps, S. J., & Morehouse, C. A. (1969). Effects of mental practice on the acquisition of motor skills of varied difficulty. Research Quarterly, 40, 773–778.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rakestraw, P. G., Irby, D. M., & Vontver, L. A. (1983). The use of mental practice in pelvic examination instruction. Journal of Medical Education, 58, 335–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rawlings, E. I., & Rawlings, I. L. (1974). Rotary pursuit tracking following mental rehearsal as a function of voluntary control of visual imagery. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 38, 302.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A. (1997). Learning from worked-out examples: A study on individual differences. Cognitive Science, 21, 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A. (1999). Learning mathematics from worked-out examples: Analyzing and fostering self-explanations. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 14, 477–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A., Stark, R., Gruber, H., & Mandl, H. (1998). Learning from worked-out examples: The effects of example variability and elicited self-explanations. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 23, 90–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Romero, K., & Silvestri, L. (1990). The role of mental practice in the acquisition and performance of motor skills. Journal of Instructional Psychology, 17, 218–221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sackett, R. S. (1934). The influence of symbolic rehearsal upon the retention of a maze habit. The Journal of General Psychology, 10, 376–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sackett, R. S. (1935). The relationship between amount of symbolic rehearsal and retention of a maze habit. The Journal of General Psychology, 13, 113–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schirmer, B. R. (2003). Using verbal protocols to identify the reading strategies of students who are deaf. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 8, 157–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shick, M. (1970). Effects of mental practice on selected volleyball skills for college women. Research Quarterly, 41, 88–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Surburg, P. R. (1968). Audio, visual and audio-visual instruction with mental practice in developing the forehand tennis drive. Research Quarterly, 39, 728–734.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tindall-Ford, S., & Sweller, J. (2006). Altering the modality of instructions to facilitate imagination: Interactions between the modality and imagination effects. Instructional Science, 34, 343–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ungerleider, S., & Golding, J. M. (1991). Mental practice among Olympic athletes. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 72, 1007–1017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Meer, J. P., & Theunissen, N. C. M. (2009). Prospective educational applications of mental simulation: A meta-review. Educational Psychology Review, 21, 93–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • VanLehn, K., Jones, R. M., & Chi, M. T. (1992). A model of the self-explanation effect. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 2, 1–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John Sweller .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2011 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Sweller, J., Ayres, P., Kalyuga, S. (2011). Facilitating Effective Mental Processes: The Imagination and Self-Explanation Effects. In: Cognitive Load Theory. Explorations in the Learning Sciences, Instructional Systems and Performance Technologies, vol 1. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-8126-4_14

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics