Skip Navigation
Skip to contents

JPMPH : Journal of Preventive Medicine and Public Health

OPEN ACCESS
SEARCH
Search

Articles

Page Path
HOME > J Prev Med Public Health > Volume 46(Suppl 1); 2013 > Article
Special Article
Mental Exercises for Cognitive Function: Clinical Evidence
Ryuta Kawashima
Journal of Preventive Medicine and Public Health 2013;46(Suppl 1):S22-S27.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.3961/jpmph.2013.46.S.S22
Published online: January 30, 2013
  • 12,580 Views
  • 148 Download
  • 26 Crossref
  • 27 Scopus

Smart Ageing International Research Center, Institute for Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan.

Corresponding author: Ryuta Kawashima, MD. 4-1 Seiryocho, Aobaku, Sendai 980-8575, Japan. Tel: +81-11-717-7988, Fax: +81-11-717-7988, ryuta@idac.tohoku.ac.jp
• Received: November 30, 2011   • Accepted: March 8, 2012

Copyright © 2013 The Korean Society for Preventive Medicine

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/3.0/) which permits unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

  • The purpose of this study was to examine the beneficial effects of a new cognitive intervention program designed for the care and prevention of dementia, namely Learning Therapy. The training program used systematized basic problems in arithmetic and Japanese language as training tasks. In study 1, 16 individuals in the experimental group and 16 in the control group were recruited from a nursing home. In both groups, all individuals were clinically diagnosed with senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. In study 2, we performed a single-blind, randomized controlled trial in our cognitive intervention program of 124 community-dwelling seniors. In both studies, the daily training program using reading and arithmetic tasks was carried out approximately 5 days a week, for 15 to 20 minutes a day in the intervention groups. Neuropsychological measures were determined simultaneously in the groups both prior to and after six months of the intervention. The results of our investigations indicate that our cognitive intervention using reading and arithmetic problems demonstrated a transfer effect and they provide convincing evidence that cognitive training maintains and improves the cognitive functions of dementia patients and healthy seniors.
In Japan, the percent of the population of elderly people age 65 and over was 23.1% in 2010, an unprecedentedly high percentage, making Japan the world's first super-aging society. This super-aging environment necessitates the formation of a society in which all individuals can play an active role, even as they grow older. Ideally, it should be a society in which people can share their wisdom and knowledge, regardless of their age or gender, in order to maintain and improve the quality of life of all people and to maintain a healthy society.
Generally, an individual's quality of life increases with age; however, events like retirement can trigger a loss in one's sense of purpose in life and a feeling of disconnection from society, in turn lessening the quality of life. However, we believe that quality of life can be improved to one's last moments if the following four factors are given due consideration and if a social system dedicates its resources to achieving them for its population: cognitive stimulation, regular exercise, balanced nutrition, and a relationship with society.
To support the establishment of a social system that aims to realize these goals, we have been developing cognitive intervention programs to promote healthy, long lives so that individuals can continue to be intellectually stimulated, find their places as integral members of society, and maintain and improve their quality of life and mental and physical health.
Cognitive training is receiving increasing interest as a solution to age-related cognitive decline. Although the general public's interest in cognitive training, or mental exercises, is increasing, the generalizing or transfer effect of such training remains unclear. With regard to the changes in cognitive functions aging induces, it is important to note that while cognitive functions related to semantic knowledge do not decline with age, all cognitive functions are dependent on the prefrontal cortex, particularly the executive function, which declines linearly with age [1,2]. We previously demonstrated that cognitive intervention programs using intensive adoptive training of working memory [3,4] induce a plastic change in the brain structures of healthy young subjects in addition to improving their non-trained cognitive functions (transfer effects). Training which uses cognitive tasks, including working memory tasks, has been shown to improve performance on trained tasks as well as some untrained transfer tasks related to memory, intelligence, and response inhibition (for a review, [5]). Also, while the lateral prefrontal and parietal regions play a key role in working memory [6], studies demonstrated altered patterns of brain activity during untrained cognitive tasks, altered density of cortical dopamine D1 receptors, and altered white matter integrity after training on some working memory (WM) tasks associated with the prefrontal and parietal regions (for a review, [7]).
The purpose of this study was to examine the beneficial effects of Learning Therapy, a new cognitive intervention program designed for the care [8] and prevention of dementia [9]. The concepts of Learning Therapy are derived from our working memory training program, the tasks of which include systematized basic problems in arithmetic and Japanese language (Figure 1). The task of reading aloud is accomplished with a combination of several cognitive processes, for example, recognition of visually presented words, conversion of these graphic representations to phonological representations, analysis of the word meanings, and control of pronunciation. Solving arithmetic problems is also a task that cannot be accomplished without many cognitive processes, for example, recognition of visually presented numbers, performance of arithmetic operations, and control of hand movement. Both tasks are known to activate various brain areas including the prefrontal cortex of the bilateral hemispheres [10,11], in addition to other brain areas, and both clearly require working memory. Also, both tasks are simple and easy, so even people with senile dementia are able to understand, perform, and continue doing the tasks prepared in our program.
Study 1
Sixteen individuals in the experimental group and 16 in the control group were recruited from a nursing home. Subjects were randomly assigned to these two groups. All of the individuals in both groups had a clinical diagnosis of senile dementia of the Alzheimer type [8]. None of the participants showed abnormalities such as tumors, cerebral bleeding, cerebral infarction, or pathological brain atrophy on an X-ray computed tomography examination. The baseline clinical and neuropsychological characteristics of the experimental and control participants are summarized in Table 1.
Study 2
We performed a single-blind, randomized controlled trial on our cognitive intervention program in community-dwelling seniors living in the same local community in Sendai, Japan [9]. In the first phase of the survey, we sent out information regarding the present study to all district residents ages 70 and over (total 3136; 1301 men, and 1853 women). We invited all these individuals to participate in a comprehensive geriatric assessment which included an assessment of medical and dental statuses, physical functions, and cognitive functions. We distributed leaflets describing the purpose and plans for this study to 962 individuals from which 269 applied for the study. The inclusion criteria were a Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [12] score greater than 23 and a Geriatric Depression Scale score less than 15. Applicants who had a history of cerebrovascular diseases, who were active in another cognitive-related training program, and/or who were unavailable during the study period were excluded. In the end, 124 participants satisfying the inclusion and exclusion criteria were allocated into either the experimental group (n=62) or the control group (n=62) by random drawing using a computer.
In both studies, the daily training program using reading and arithmetic tasks was carried out in the intervention groups approximately 5 days a week, 15 to 20 minutes a day for six months. We prepared a wide range of materials (Figure 1).
In the first study, the lowest level arithmetic task was counting practice and the highest level was three-digit division. For the Japanese language tasks, the lowest level was reading and writing single syllables and the highest level was reading fairy tales aloud. The participants came to the learning center either on summons by the staff or by their own initiative every day from Monday to Saturday. Task worksheets of each task were prepared for each participant, each day, and participants would complete two to five of these worksheets daily. The participants' work was assessed by the Learning Center's staff and any mistakes were corrected by the participants. If the participants could not solve a problem by themselves, the staff members stationed between desks provided sufficient advice for them to be able to solve the problems. The study period ended when the participants completed each of the problems correctly.
In the second study, the lowest level of arithmetic-task difficulty was single-digit addition and the highest was three-digit division. For the Japanese language task, the lowest level of difficulty was reading and writing simple sentences and the highest level was reading fairy tales aloud. Prior to the training program, the appropriate degree of difficulty and workload for each subject was assessed by a diagnostic test, so that participants could continue to perform the learning tasks with ease. The participants in the experimental group were asked to go to classes in two elementary schools once a week. At the schools, five task worksheets of each task were prepared for each participant and the participants' work was assessed by the Learning Center's staff. Any mistakes were corrected by the participants. The study period ended when the participants completed each of the problems correctly.
Neuropsychological measures were determined simultaneously in both the intervention and control groups prior to and six months after the intervention (post-test) by a MMSE [12], and Frontal Assessment Battery at bedside (FAB) [13]; therefore, the time span between the pre- and post-tests was six months. As for the second study, the Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DST) of the WAIS-R [14] was performed in addition to the MMSE and FAB. The neuropsychological characteristics of both groups were compared using an independent sample t-test at baseline and during follow up. A further analysis comparing baseline and follow-up scores separately for the learning and control groups was performed with a paired t-test. Statistical significance was set at p<0.05 for all of the comparisons.
Study1
The scores of the neuropsychological characteristics at the baseline and follow up are summarized in Figure 2. In the learning group, the FAB score showed a statistically significant (p<0.05) increase after the training had taken place. However, the MMSE score did not show any significant changes. In the control group, the MMSE score showed a statistically significant (p<0.05) decrease from baseline to follow up. Nevertheless, the FAB score did not show any significant changes. During the follow up, the MMSE and the FAB scores of the learning group were statistically significantly (p<0.05) higher than those of the control group. There was no relationship between the magnitude of change seen in the neuropsychological characteristics and initial scores on the MMSE or FAB in the learning group.
Study 2
The scores of the neuropsychological characteristics at baseline and follow up are summarized in Figure 3. In the experimental group, the FAB and DST scores showed a statistically significant (p<0.001) increase after the intervention. However, the MMSE score showed no significant changes. In the control group, the FAB and DST scores showed no significant change. The MMSE score decreased from the pre- to post-test; however, the decrease did not reach a statistically significant level (p=0.055). In the post-test, the MMSE score of the experimental group was higher than that of the control group. However, the difference did not reach a statistically significant level (p=0.05). The DST score showed no significant difference between the experimental and control groups in the post-test.
The results of these studies were convincing evidence that a daily training program involving reading and arithmetic problems has an immediate and beneficial effect on mental processing speed (as measured by DST) and executive function (as measured by FAB). These effects are also not directly tied to the intervention. Participants maintained their improvements six months after the training program.
Our intervention program is based on a system of daily training. Other previous cognitive intervention studies used a relatively short intervention period compared with the present study and also found long-term beneficial effects on cognitive functions. Previous intervention studies by the ACTIVE trial [15] and Mahncke et al. [16] showed improvements in cognitive functions. The results of many clinical studies of humans and experimental studies of animals support the idea that continuous stimulation of the brain causes changes in the brain. There is no doubt that the brain has a lifelong capacity for plasticity, which is why inducing changes in brain networks with a daily intervention program is a reasonable method of stimulation. The sustained beneficial effects on the non-directly targeted cognitive functions found in this study and in other intervention studies [16] may be related to the same mechanism. Although we found sustained high cognitive activity six months after the intervention, further observation to estimate the longevity of the improvement seen in the intervention is necessary.
In general, aging is viewed negatively as the loss of something that people have in their youth or as a form of regression. As a result, society has formed a false image that aging is something like an illness, that it is ugly, and that young people are superior to the elderly in many respects. However, we consider aging to mean that people can grow and become wiser as they reach the later stages of life. We call this concept "smart aging" and advocate it as a healthy outlook for society to adopt.
Events like retirement can trigger a loss of one's sense of purpose in life and initiate a feeling of disconnection from society as one grows in age, which in turn lessens the quality of life. However, we believe that the quality of life can be improved to one's last moments if the following four factors are given due consideration and if a social system dedicates its resources to achieving them for its population: cognitive stimulation, regular exercise, balanced nutrition, and a relationship with society.
Here, we proposed a smart, but inexpensive system for cognitive stimulation. The results of our investigations demonstrate that reading and solving arithmetic problems in our cognitive intervention program indeed has a transfer effect, and they are convincing evidence that cognitive training maintains and improves the cognitive functions of dementia patients and healthy seniors.
I thank professor Ichiro Tsuji at the Graduate School of Medicine, Tohoku University, for his suggestions and collaboration during the experimental setup and design. I also thank all the staff members involved in the Tsurugaya Project, and Kumon Learning Center for their technical support of the cognitive measures and intervention. This study was supported by RISTEX, JST, and a collaborative study between Tohoku University and Sendai City (Gakuto collaboration research project).

The author has no conflicts of interest with the material presented in this paper.

  • 1. Park DC, Lautenschlager G, Hedden T, Davidson NS, Smith AD, Smith PK. Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychol Aging 2002;17(2):299-320. 12061414ArticlePubMed
  • 2. Salthouse TA. Mental exercise and mental aging: evaluating the validity of the "use it or lose it" hypothesis. Perspect Psychol Sci 2006;1(1):68-87ArticlePubMed
  • 3. Takeuchi H, Sekiguchi A, Taki Y, Yokoyama S, Yomogida Y, Komuro N, et al. Training of working memory impacts structural connectivity. J Neurosci 2010;30(9):3297-3303. 20203189ArticlePubMedPMC
  • 4. Takeuchi H, Taki Y, Sassa Y, Hashizume H, Sekiguchi A, Fukushima A, et al. Working memory training using mental calculation impacts regional gray matter of the frontal and parietal regions. PLoS One 2011;6(8):e23175. 21886781ArticlePubMedPMC
  • 5. Perrig WJ, Hollenstein M, Oelhafen S. Can we improve fluid intelligence with training on working memory in persons with intellectual disabilities? J Cogn Educ Psychol 2009;8(2):148-164Article
  • 6. Baddeley A. Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nat Rev Neurosci 2003;4(10):829-839. 14523382ArticlePubMed
  • 7. Takeuchi H, Taki Y, Kawashima R. Effects of working memory training on cognitive functions and neural systems. Rev Neurosci 2010;21(6):427-449. 21438192ArticlePubMed
  • 8. Kawashima R, Okita K, Yamazaki R, Tajima N, Yoshida H, Taira M, et al. Reading aloud and arithmetic calculation improve frontal function of people with dementia. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2005;60(3):380-384. 15860478ArticlePubMed
  • 9. Uchida S, Kawashima R. Reading and solving arithmetic problems improves cognitive functions of normal aged people: a randomized controlled study. Age (Dordr) 2008;30(1):21-29. 19424870ArticlePubMedPMC
  • 10. Kawashima R, Taira M, Okita K, Inoue K, Tajima N, Yoshida H, et al. A functional MRI study of simple arithmetic-a comparison between children and adults. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2004;18(3):227-233. 14741309ArticlePubMed
  • 11. Miura N, Watanabe J, Iwata K, Sassa Y, Riera J, Tsuchiya H, et al. Cortical activation during reading of ancient versus modern Japanese texts: fMRI study. Neuroimage 2005;26(2):426-431. 15907300ArticlePubMed
  • 12. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-mental state. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12(3):189-198. 1202204ArticlePubMed
  • 13. Dubois B, Slachevsky A, Litvan I, Pillon B. The FAB: a Frontal Assessment Battery at bedside. Neurology 2000;55(11):1621-1626. 11113214ArticlePubMed
  • 14. Wechsler D. Manual for the Wechsler adult intelligence scale: revised. 1981. New York: The Psychological Corporation
  • 15. Ball K, Berch DB, Helmers KF, Jobe JB, Leveck MD, Marsiske M, et al. Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA 2002;288(18):2271-2281. 12425704ArticlePubMedPMC
  • 16. Mahncke HW, Connor BB, Appelman J, Ahsanuddin ON, Hardy JL, Wood RA, et al. Memory enhancement in healthy older adults using a brain plasticity-based training program: a randomized, controlled study. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006;103(33):12523-12528. 16888038ArticlePubMedPMC
Figure 1
Examples of the materials (English version). (A) Reading materials and (B) arithmetic materials.
jpmph-46-S22-g001.jpg
Figure 2
Changes neuropsychological characteristics for study 1. All of the subjects were patients with dementia of the Alzheimer's type. (A) Mini-Mental Status Examination score and (B) Frontal Assessment Battery at bedside score. Error bars indicate the standard error of the mean. ns, not significant.
jpmph-46-S22-g002.jpg
Figure 3
Changes in neuropsychological characteristics for study 2. All the subjects were community dwelling elderly people. (A) Mini-Mental Status Examination score, (B) Frontal Assessment Battery at bedside score, and (C) Digit Symbol Substitution Test of the WAIS-R score. Error bars indicate the standard error of the mean.
jpmph-46-S22-g003.jpg
Table 1.
Clinical and neuropsychological characteristics of the experimental and control groups of study 1
Experimental (n = 16) Control (n = 16)
Mean age (y) 85.1 (5.4) 86.3 (4.9)
Age range (y) 76-96 78-96
Mean MMSE score 19.9 (7.0) 19.6 (5.4)
MMSE range 7-30 7-29
Mean FAB score 7.0 (2.8) 6.8 (3.2)
FAB range 3-14 3-12

Numbers in parentheses indicate standard deviation.

MMSE, Mini-Mental Status Examination; FAB, Frontal Assessment Battery at bedside.

Figure & Data

References

    Citations

    Citations to this article as recorded by  
    • Integrated training sessions by a neuropsychologist and a speech therapist in the rehabilitation of persons in chronic critical condition by the example of FRCC ICMR
      Ekaterina A. Kharitoshkina, Andrey V. Grechko, Elena V. Bushueva
      Physical and rehabilitation medicine, medical rehabilitation.2023; 5(1): 30.     CrossRef
    • Effectiveness of an enhanced simultaneous cognitive-physical dual-task training based on fairy tales (ESCARF) in older adults with mild cognitive impairment
      Younkyoung Kim, Saeryun Kim, Hyunju Yang
      Geriatric Nursing.2023; 53: 57.     CrossRef
    • The utility and cultural specificity of cognitive-based interventions in preventing cognitive decline among healthy older adults in Asia: A systematic review
      Prashanth Vasantha Kumar, Ponnusamy Subramaniam, Normah Che Din
      Asian Journal of Psychiatry.2021; 60: 102646.     CrossRef
    • Alien Attack: A Non-Pharmaceutical Complement for ADHD Treatment
      Sofia Ahufinger, Pilar Herrero-Martín
      Entropy.2021; 23(10): 1321.     CrossRef
    • Análisis del efecto de un programa de estimulación cognitiva en personas con envejecimiento normal en Atención Primaria: ensayo clínico aleatorizado
      Estela Calatayud, Fernando Plo, Carmen Muro
      Atención Primaria.2020; 52(1): 38.     CrossRef
    • Bayesian analysis of the association between effective strategies of multimodal nonpharmacological intervention and characteristics of cognitive function in nursing home residents with cognitive impairment
      Kyosuke Yorozuya, Shingo Yamane, Misako Nobuhisa, Hiroko Owaki, Takeaki Suzuki, Hikaru Okahara, Wataru Iwamori, Hideaki Hanaoka
      Medicine.2020; 99(37): e22154.     CrossRef
    • Effective strategies for prevention of cardiovascular complications in elderly patients with arterial hypertension and obesity (clinical example)
      A. P. Pereverzev, O. D. Ostroumova, E. V. Mironova
      Medical alphabet.2020; (21): 10.     CrossRef
    • Does social interaction influence the effect of cognitive intervention program? A randomized controlled trial using Go game
      Ai Iizuka, Hiroyuki Suzuki, Susumu Ogawa, Kimi Estela Kobayashi‐Cuya, Momoko Kobayashi, Hiroki Inagaki, Mika Sugiyama, Shuichi Awata, Toru Takebayashi, Yoshinori Fujiwara
      International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry.2019; 34(2): 324.     CrossRef
    • Research on intelligent cognitive function enhancement of intelligent robot based on ant colony algorithm
      Yang Jian, Yang Li
      Cognitive Systems Research.2019; 56: 203.     CrossRef
    • Cognitive training for people with mild to moderate dementia
      Alex Bahar-Fuchs, Anthony Martyr, Anita MY Goh, Julieta Sabates, Linda Clare
      Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews.2019;[Epub]     CrossRef
    • Non-drug therapies for cognitive impairment
      I. S. Preobrazhenskaya, D. Fantalis, S. A. Abdyshova, A. A. Kindarova
      Neurology, Neuropsychiatry, Psychosomatics.2019; 11(3S): 68.     CrossRef
    • Virtual training leads to real acute physical, cognitive, and neural benefits on healthy adults: study protocol for a randomized controlled trial
      Dalila Burin, Noriki Yamaya, Rie Ogitsu, Ryuta Kawashima
      Trials.2019;[Epub]     CrossRef
    • Kognitive Interventionen in Pflegeheimen
      C. Wöhl, S. Richter, B. Blättner
      Zeitschrift für Gerontologie und Geriatrie.2018; 51(6): 656.     CrossRef
    • Pilot Randomized Controlled Trial of the GO Game Intervention on Cognitive Function
      Ai Iizuka, Hiroyuki Suzuki, Susumu Ogawa, Kimi Estela Kobayashi-Cuya, Momoko Kobayashi, Toru Takebayashi, Yoshinori Fujiwara
      American Journal of Alzheimer's Disease & Other Dementias®.2018; 33(3): 192.     CrossRef
    • Cognitive and motor training for patients with moderate cognitive impairment and mild dementia
      A. A. Naumenko, I. S. Preobrazhenskaya
      Neurology, Neuropsychiatry, Psychosomatics.2018; 10(4): 81.     CrossRef
    • Effects of a Cooking Program Based on Brain-activating Rehabilitation for Elderly Residents with Dementia in a Roken Facility: A Randomized Controlled Trial
      Tatsuhiko Murai, Haruyasu Yamaguchi
      Progress in Rehabilitation Medicine.2017; 2: n/a.     CrossRef
    • Actively station: Effects on global cognition of mature adults and healthy elderly program using eletronic games
      Tiago Nascimento Ordonez, Felipe Borges, Camila Sato Kanashiro, Carolina Carneiro das Neves Santos, Samara Santos Hora, Thais Bento Lima-Silva
      Dementia & Neuropsychologia.2017; 11(2): 186.     CrossRef
    • Dementia Care by Healthy Elderly Caregivers Is Associated with Improvement of Patients' Memory and the Caregivers' Quality of Life: A Before and After Study
      Seung-Gul Kang, Kyoung-Sae Na, Jae Myeong Kang, Byeong Kil Yeon, Jun-Young Lee, Seong-Jin Cho
      Psychiatry Investigation.2017; 14(4): 458.     CrossRef
    • Cognitive interventions in patients with dementia living in long-term care facilities: Systematic review and meta-analysis
      Ann-Kristin Folkerts, Mandy Roheger, Jeremy Franklin, Jennifer Middelstädt, Elke Kalbe
      Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics.2017; 73: 204.     CrossRef
    • Mantenimiento y conservación de la memoria en un grupo de ancianos
      Calos E. Rodríguez-Angarita
      Repertorio de Medicina y Cirugía.2017; 26(4): 219.     CrossRef
    • SAIDO Learning as a Cognitive Intervention for Dementia Care: A Preliminary Study
      Ryuta Kawashima, Deborah Lewis Hiller, Sheryl L. Sereda, Michelle Antonczak, Kara Serger, Denise Gannon, Shinji Ito, Hiroshi Otake, Daisaku Yunomae, Akihito Kobayashi, Christopher Muller, Hiroyuki Murata, Stephanie FallCreek
      Journal of the American Medical Directors Association.2015; 16(1): 56.     CrossRef
    • Improving Cognitive Function from Children to Old Age: A Systematic Review of Recent Smart Ageing Intervention Studies
      Rui Nouchi, Ryuta Kawashima
      Advances in Neuroscience.2014; 2014: 1.     CrossRef
    • 4. Front Line of Rehabilitation for People with Dementia
      Yoshiki Takao
      Nihon Naika Gakkai Zasshi.2014; 103(8): 1839.     CrossRef
    • Brain training games improve cognitive functions in the healthy elderly
      Rui Nouchi, Ryuta Kawashima
      Higher Brain Function Research.2014; 34(3): 335.     CrossRef
    • Physical Activity for Health: Evidence, Theory, and Practice
      Jin-Jong Chen, Yunhwan Lee
      Journal of Preventive Medicine & Public Health.2013; 46(Suppl 1): S1.     CrossRef
    • Implication of the Nutritional and Nonnutritional Factors in the Context of Preservation of Cognitive Performance in Patients With Dementia/Depression and Alzheimer Disease
      Gjumrakch Aliev, Ghulam Md Ashraf, Yury G. Kaminsky, Ishfaq Ahmed Sheikh, Sergey K. Sudakov, Nikolay N. Yakhno, Valery V. Benberin, Sergey O. Bachurin
      American Journal of Alzheimer's Disease & Other Dementiasr.2013; 28(7): 660.     CrossRef

    Figure

    JPMPH : Journal of Preventive Medicine and Public Health