Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

The effect of hearing aids on cognitive function: A systematic review

  • Maxime E. Sanders ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Maxime E. Sanders, Ellen Kant

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Department of Otorhinolaryngology and Head & Neck Surgery, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands

  • Ellen Kant ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Maxime E. Sanders, Ellen Kant

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Department of Otorhinolaryngology and Head & Neck Surgery, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands

  • Adriana L. Smit,

    Roles Conceptualization, Supervision

    Affiliations Department of Otorhinolaryngology and Head & Neck Surgery, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands, Brain Center, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands

  • Inge Stegeman

    Roles Conceptualization, Supervision

    I.Stegeman@umcutrecht.nl

    Affiliations Department of Otorhinolaryngology and Head & Neck Surgery, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands, Brain Center, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands, Department of Ophthalmology, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands, Epidemiology and Data Science, Amsterdam University Medical Centers, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

Abstract

Rationale

Dementia currently affects 50 million people globally with this expected to triple by 2050. Even though hearing loss is associated with cognitive decline, the underlying mechanisms are not fully understood. Considering hearing loss is the largest modifiable risk factor for developing dementia, it is essential to study the effect of hearing aids on cognitive function.

Objective

To systematically review the existing literature to examine the evidence for using hearing aids intervention as a treatment for deteriorating cognitive function.

Design

A search of PubMed, Cochrane Library, Embase and grey literature was conducted revealing 3060 unique records between 1990–2020. Two reviewers independently selected longitudinal studies observing the effects of hearing aids on cognitive function in persons without dementia at onset of the study. Due to the heterogeneity of the data, a meta-analysis could not be performed. Outcomes are described in a summary of findings table and portrayed diagrammatically.

Results

We identified 17 unique studies, spanning 30 years of research and 3526 participants. The included studies made use of 50 different cognitive function tests. These tests were grouped into separate cognitive domains according to the DSM-V classification for further analysis. The most beneficial impact of hearing aids seems to be in the cognitive domain of executive function, with six studies showing improvement, two studies being inconclusive and three studies not demonstrating a significant effect. Three of five studies demonstrated significant improvement when screening for brief mental status. The least beneficial impact is seen in domain of complex attention, with eight studies showing no significant effects, compared with one demonstrating improvement with intervention.

Conclusions

Based on this systematic review, we conclude that there is controversy about the effects of hearing aids on cognition. Additional research through randomized clinical trials with standardized cognitive assessment and longer follow-up is warranted to further elucidate this relationship.

Introduction

Dementia affected 50 million people worldwide in 2018, with this number expected to increase to 150 million by the year 2050 [1]. Dementia is a disorder characterized by slowly progressing cognitive decline [2] that interferes with normal daily functioning and independence [3]. The history of cognitive decline and dementia can be dated back to the ancient Greek and Roman times and was long thought to be an inevitable part of life, neither preventable nor treatable [4]. However, our understanding of cognitive decline is rapidly shifting with more insights gained into aetiology of the clinical syndrome. In 2017, the Lancet Commission for dementia prevention, intervention, and care described a model of life-course risk factors that influence the progression to dementia [3]. It is estimated that 40% of these risk factors are modifiable and could provide an opportunity for early prevention and disease progression [5]. According to the most recent report of the 2020 Lancet Commission for dementia prevention, intervention, and care, hearing loss accounts for up to 8.2% of the risk factors of dementia and is thereby the largest potentially modifiable risk factor identified [5].

Hearing loss is a public health problem affecting over 466 million people globally [6]. According to the Global Burden of Disease Study, hearing loss has become the third highest cause of years lived with disability [7]. The prevalence of hearing loss increases with older age, with an estimation by the WHO that one-third of adults older than 65 years of age experience disabling hearing loss (>40 dB at the average of 500, 1000, 2000 and 4000Hz in the better ear) [68]. Hearing loss is associated with reduced quality of life and can result in loneliness [9], social isolation, lowered mood, depression [10], anxiety and poorer physical health [1113]. Conventional hearing aids are the primary treatment for hearing loss and are effective to restore hearing. Despite this, hearing aids are significantly underutilized with less than 20% of those with a hearing impairment using them [14].

It is estimated that if interventions have the ability to delay the onset of dementia by 1 year, this would decrease the global prevalence of dementia by 10% [15]. Hearing aids are a relatively low-cost intervention when compared to the high societal and psychosocial costs that accompany declining cognitive ability. Considering that hearing loss often precedes the onset of clinical dementia by at least 5 to 10 years [8], it is imperative to investigate the role of hearing intervention on cognitive outcome before the onset of dementia. Henceforth, the focus of this review will be on the stage before dementia, cognitive decline. Even though there is a strong association between hearing loss and cognitive decline [1618], the underlying mechanisms linking the two are not fully elucidated. Previous systematic reviews have focused largely on the association between hearing loss and cognitive function and while some commented on the intervention effects of hearing aids, there was no clear consensus [5, 11, 19, 20]. The purpose of this study is to systematically review the literature on prospective cohort studies examining the intervention effect of hearing aid usage on cognitive function in adults who have not been diagnosed with dementia.

Methods

This review is reported according to Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis [21]. The protocol for this review is pre-registered on the PROSPERO International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (CRD42020171872) [22].

Eligibility criteria

Inclusion criteria included published and ongoing longitudinal studies describing the effect of hearing aid treatment on cognitive function. Studies were only included when participants were declared first-time hearing aid users. Study populations with diagnosed dementia at onset of study and duplicate data sets were excluded.

Search strategy and information sources

The search terms “hearing aids” and “cognition” with their synonyms were used (S1 File. Syntax search string) We conducted an extensive, literature search in electronic databases (PubMed, Cochrane Library, Embase) on 11 November 2020. Grey literature was searched using two electronic databases: ClinicalTrials.gov [23] and TrialRegister.nl [24]. We supplemented the electronic search by the manual searches of the references from articles and review papers. We contacted authors of ongoing studies or articles whose full-text or data were not yet available for unpublished work.

Study selection

The records were independently screened by two reviewers (MS and EK). Relevant articles identified in the title and abstract screening were further assessed in full-text screening and assessed for suitability according to the inclusion and exclusion criteria. Conflicts were resolved by discussion and in the case of disagreement a third researcher (IS or AS) weighed in.

Data collection process and analysis

The following relevant data were extracted: study ID, study location, study design, number of participants, age and gender of participants, definition of the hearing loss, assessment method of hearing loss, average hearing loss at baseline, follow-up time and outcome measures concerning cognition. Due to heterogeneity of the cognitive tests used and length of follow-up, a meta-analysis could not be performed. Instead, the cognitive outcomes are described in a summary of findings table. When available, the control group data and either a probability value or confidence interval were included. We grouped all outcomes of individual cognitive tests into five (of six) neurocognitive domains according to the DMS-V classification: perceptual motor function, executive function, complex attention, language, learning and memory and two broader domains; brief mental status and general intelligence [25, 26]. Note that the social cognition domain (the sixth domain according to DSM-V) was not tested for in the studies reviewed. These data were portrayed diagrammatically. The effect was expressed as either significantly improving, impeding or having no significant effect on cognition.

Risk of bias assessment

Two reviewers (MS and EK) independently critically appraised the studies for quality. The Cochrane Risk of Bias tool [27] was used to assess randomized controlled trials (RCTs), while the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale [28] was used for non-randomized prospective cohort studies. The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale was converted to good, fair or poor quality according to the Agency for Health Research and Quality standards. A study was considered of good quality in case of 3 or 4 stars in selection domain and 1 or 2 stars in comparability domain and 2 or 3 stars in outcome/exposure domain. A study was considered of fair quality in case of 2 stars in selection domain and 1 or 2 stars in comparability domain and 2 or 3 stars in outcome/exposure domain. A study was considered of poor quality in the case of 0 or 1 star in selection domain or 0 stars in comparability domain or 0 or 1 stars in outcome/exposure domain. Disagreements between the reviewers were resolved by discussion or input from a third researcher (IS or AS).

Results

Search strategy

The results of the search generated 4027 records, with an additional 7 found through grey literature searches and snowballing. Exclusion of duplicates left 3060 unique records. Title and abstract screening revealed 88 potentially relevant studies. One additional study was added through correspondence with authors. The full-text screening identified 17 relevant studies (Fig 1).

Study design characteristics

Table 1 outlines the characteristics of the included studies. The 17 included studies span from 1990 to 2020, 4 of which are RCTs [2932], and the other 13 are prospective cohort studies. The study sample sizes ranged from 6 to 2040 participants, totalling 3526 participants in this systematic review. The studies were situated largely in high income countries. The duration of follow-up differed from 6 weeks to 14 years. With six of the studies using a follow-up of 6 months [30, 31, 33, 36, 41, 43]. In nine studies the control group consisted of hearing-impaired participants without hearing aid treatment [29, 3138]. The other eight studies compared the effect of the intervention results to baseline results [30, 3945]. The studies by Deal et al. [30] and Brewster et al. [32] are primarily feasibility studies and secondarily assessed the cognitive outcomes over a period of 6 months and 12 weeks respectively. In the study of Deal et al. [30] the comparator group participated in the “successful aging intervention”, an interactive health education program which did not meet the inclusion criteria for this review. Therefore, for this review, the intervention outcomes were compared with baseline outcomes for the hearing aid group. In the study of Castiglione et al. [42] only group A met the inclusion criteria for this review. The 17 included studies made use of 50 different cognitive function tests, none of which are used across the board. These outcome measures covered the following cognitive domains: brief mental status, general intelligence, perceptual motor function, executive function, complex attention, language, learning and memory.

Assessment of risk of bias and representativeness

The risk of bias assessments for the RCTs and the prospective cohort studies are shown in Tables 2 and 3 respectively. Three RCTs are judged to have some concerns [3032] and one as high risk for bias [29].

thumbnail
Table 2. Cochrane risk of bias tool for randomized trials.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0261207.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Newcastle-Ottawa Scale for assessing non-randomized trials.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0261207.t003

In all studies, participants were randomised using appropriate random sequence methods, however one trial had baseline imbalances with a significantly younger intervention group and therefore was rated with some concerns [32]. We judged the deviations from the intended intervention to be high in one study [29], with some concerns in two studies [30, 31]. In these studies blinding was not performed and in one study [29] there was insufficient assignment to the intervention, because 15% of persons in intervention group reported wearing their aids less than four hours daily. The deviations from intended intervention was judged to be low in one study [32], because of the use of sham hearing aids. We judged a low risk of bias in missing outcome data in all the RCTs [2932], because data were available for nearly all participants. The measurement of outcome was considered to be a high risk of bias in one study [29], because of the low sensitivity and specificity of the use of Short Portable Mental Status Questionnaire. The measurement of outcome was rated with some concern of bias in one study [31], because of potential influence on performance due to a difference in the amount of contact moments between the exposed and non-exposed group. The measurement of outcome was judged a low risk in two studies [30, 32]. The selection of the reported result was judged low risk in three studies [3032] and some concerns in one study [29], because in this study only one of the multiple analyses was reported numerically.

The 13 cohort studies were assessed using the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale and 1 is rated as good quality [34], 2 rated as fair quality [36, 44] and 10 rated as poor quality [33, 35, 3743, 45]. The item selection of the non-exposed cohort was assessed with a high risk of bias in 11 of the 13 studies [35, 3745], as these non-exposed participants were all recruited from a different source. There was insufficient data on the ascertainment of exposure in five studies [35, 36, 39, 40, 42], mainly because they did not specify how they measured adherence to hearing aid use. Additionally, there was insufficient data on outcome of exposure in seven studies [33, 3538, 40, 42], because they did not demonstrate that cognitive decline was not present at the start of the study. On the item comparability, two studies were assessed with a high risk of bias [41, 43], five studies got rated with some concerns [33, 36, 37, 39, 42] and six studies got rated with a low risk of bias [34, 35, 38, 40, 44, 45]. The item length of follow-up had a high risk of bias, with only two studies have a follow-up longer than ten years [38, 44], which we assessed as a minimum adequate length of follow-up because of the slowly progressive nature of cognitive decline. Eleven studies were rated with a low risk of bias in the assessment of outcome [3336, 38, 39, 4145], in one study the assessment of outcome was performed by self-report [40] and another provided a non-blinded, auditory test, considered not sufficient [37].

Hearing level of the participants at baseline

Table 1 describes the mean hearing levels of the participants in the studies. In most of the studies, hearing impairment is measured by pure tone audiometry. The mean hearing level in the intervention groups varied from 31.2 (7.9) dB to 58.4dB, whereas the mean hearing level in the control groups varied from 29.8 (9.0) dB to 51 (8) dB. Regarding the studies with a control group, two studies showed intervention groups with a higher reported hearing loss level than the control group (Obuchi et al. [35] HA: 46.6(10.8)dB, HI-C: 31.5(13.7)dB; Choi et al. [36] HA: 50.3(14.7)dB, HI-C: 40.7(19.0)dB). Additionally, in two other studies this was statistically tested and a statistically significant difference of about 10dB was found (Tesch-Römer et al. [33] Hearing aid group (HA) 47.3(10.4)dB, Hearing impaired control group (HI-C) 37.8(9.6)dB, p< 0.01; Dawes et al. [38] HA: 38.9(10.5)dB, HI-C: 29.8(9.0)dB, p< 0.0001). Doherty et al. [37] stated that if a participant did not meet the hearing threshold criteria for being fit with hearing aids, they were assigned to the control group. At baseline they measured the speech recognition percentage scores and stated there were no significant differences among their participant groups. In the study by van Hooren et al. [34] the hearing levels between the two groups were not matched and the control group existed of individuals who refrained from using hearing aids.

Effect on cognition

Data of the outcomes of the cognitive tests at baseline and after intervention are provided in Table 4. Figs 2 and 3 display diagrammatically the cognitive test outcomes per study categorized in six specific cognitive domains or a generalized mental status domain. The outcomes of the studies are described per cognitive domain.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Representation of cognitive tests divided into cognitive domains for studies with control group.

A: Mulrow 1990 (A1: SPMSQ); B: Tesch-Romer 1997 (B1: DSST, B2: Digit letter test B3: Spot-a-word, B4: Letter ‘s’ test, B5: Naming animals); C: van Hooren 2005 (C1*: SCWT, C2: LDST, C3: CST, C4: VVLT-immediate and -delayed, C5: Verbal fluency test); D: Obuchi 2011 (D1: dichotic listening test, D2: WAIS-R); E: Choi 2011 (E1: VVLT total, E2: Recognition score, E3: Latency score); F: Doherty 2015 (F1: Listening span test, F2: N-back test);G: Dawes 2015 (G1: MMSE, G2: DSST, G3: Trail making test A, G4: Trail making test B, G5: Auditory verbal learning, G6: Verbal fluency test); H: Karawani 2018 (H1: Working memory, H2: Flanker, H3: Processing speed); I: Brewster 2020 (I1: RBANS-Immediate Memory, I2: RBANS-Delayed Memory, I3: RBANS-Language, I4: RBANS-Attention, I5: RBANS-Visuospatial/Constructional, I6: Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention Test) * C1 showed significant improvement in favor of control group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0261207.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Representation of cognitive tests divided into cognitive domains for studies without control group.

A: Acar 2011 (A1: MMSE); B: Magalhães 2011 (B1: MMSE); C: Boi 2012 (C1: MMSE, C2: CDT), D: Castiglione 2016 (D1: Digit Span Test); E: Desjardins 2016 (E1: Listening Span Test, E2: Reading Span Test, E3: Auditory reaction time, E4: Stroop test, E5: DSST, E6: Corpus response measure), F: Deal 2017 (F1: Delayed word recall, F2: Logical memory A, F3: Incidental learning, F4: Word fluency, F5: Boston naming test, F6: Trail making test A, F7: Trail making test B, F8: DSST); G: Maharani 2018: (G1: immediate and delayed word recall, G2: Serial 7’s, G3: date naming); H: Sarant 2020 (H 1: GML test, H2: Detection test, H3: Identification test, H4: One card learning test, H5: One back test).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0261207.g003

Brief mental status domain

Five studies tested brief mental status. Mulrow et al. [29] used the Short Portable Mental Status Questionnaire and found a significant improvement in cognitive functioning after four months of follow-up (percent improvement 0.28, 95% C.I. 0.08–0.48, p = 0.008). The Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) was used in four studies. Acar et al. [39] and Magalhães et al. [40] both found significant improvement after respectively 3- and 12-months hearing aid use compared to baseline, respectively 20.3(7.7) to 23.0(7.5), p<0.005 and 21.6(3.9) to 25.3(3.3), p<0.001. Furthermore Magalhães et al. [40] showed that this improvement was independent of age or gender. The study of Boi et al. [41] did not give information regarding significance, stating the score on the MMSE remained “substantially stable” after 12 months of hearing aid use compared to baseline (26.93(0.80) to 28.17(0.56)). Dawes et al. [38], with a study sample size of 666 participants, did not see any statistical difference between the intervention group and control group after the follow-up of 11 years (HA: 26.7(0.4) to 25.9(0.5), HI-C: 26.5(0.1) to 26.9(0.2)).

General intelligence

Obuchi et al. [35] used the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised and found that the performance task scores of the HA user group were higher than those of the non-HA user group after three years (displayed graphically in their paper).

Perceptual motor function

The detection test in the cohort study of Sarant et al. [45] did not reveal any significant differences after 18 months hearing aid use (2.58(0.08) to 2.6(0.08), p = 0.077).

Executive function

Executive functioning was evaluated in five studies with a control group. Both the Concept Shifting Task [34] and the Trail Making Test-A [38] did not show a difference after respectively 12 months and 11 years hearing aid use (HA: 28.83(8.83) to 28.70(9.79), HI-C 29.75(8.81) to 29.91(8.27), p = 0.76; HA: 65.0(7.8) HI-C: 57.5(2.4), p = 0.37). Using two auditory based tests, the listening span test and N-back test, Doherty et al. [37] showed that working memory performance significantly improved with hearing aid use after six weeks (displayed graphically in their paper). Brewster et al. [32] found a differential numerical improvement factoring active hearing aid users when testing for Immediate Memory (np-ES = 0.25), but not for the Flanker test for executive function (np-ES = 0.33). The Working Memory test used by Karawani et al. [31] significantly improved in the hearing aid group after six months (HA: 108.10(11.71) to 116.25(8.99), p = 0.008, HI-C: 109.75(13.01) to 107.43(13.91), p = 0.601).

Executive functioning was also evaluated in five studies without control groups. Maharani et al. [44], having a follow-up of 14 years, focused only on executive function, using immediate and delayed word recall tests, Serial 7’s and date naming. They showed that although episodic memory declined significantly with the addition of age, the rate of decline was significantly slower after starting hearing aid use (Before hearing aid use: β = -0.11 ± 0.00, p < .001, with hearing aid use: β = -0.03 ± 0.00, p < .001, difference coefficient: 0.08, p<0.001). Hearing aid use was significantly associated with higher memory scores. (β = 2.13 ± 0.41, p<0.001). This association was independent of risk factors for cognitive impairment. Castiglione et al. [42] found a significant improvement in Digit Span test score compared to baseline (4.80(0.91) to 5.40(0.89), p<0.05) after one month hearing rehabilitation. Sarant et al. [45] used subsets of the CogState battery and found significant improvement in Groton Maze Learning test after 18 months compared to baseline (58.81(15.53) to 51(15.35), p = 0.001), but no differences in the One back test (2.96(0.1) to 2.94(0.08), p = 0.205). Deal et al. [30] found significant improvement in Logical Memory A after six months, compared to baseline (10.5(3.6) to 13.2(3.8), p<0.001) and no improvement in the Trail Making Test-B (98(73,109.5) to 99.5(69.6, 118.5). Desjardins et al. [43] used the Listening Span Test and Reading Span Test and displayed the data of their six participants individually. Five of six of the participants improved with six months of hearing aid use in both.

Complex attention

Complex attention was assessed in six studies with control groups. After a follow-up of 12 months, van Hooren et al. [34] found that the hearing-impaired control group improved significantly better than the intervention group in the Stroop-Colour-Word Test (HA: 21.81(5.17) to 22.87(5.78), HI-C: 21.70(4.61) to 21.69(3.61), p = 0.02). However, this difference disappeared when the analysis was conducted with the “compliant group” of participants (i.e., participants who used their hearing aid at least eight hours a day). The Letter Digit Substation Test did not show significant difference after intervention (HA: 27.43(6.71) to 26.32(6.82), HI-C 26.22(7.87) to 24.60(7.65), p = 0.34). Tesch-Römer et al. [33] showed no statistical significant differences between the intervention- and control group over a period of six months across Digit Symbol Substitution Test and Digit letter test (respectively HA: 40.1(11.1) to 41.0(12.1), p>0.05, HI-C 43.1(120) to 43.6(13.0) p>0.05; HA: 106.5(23.9) to 109.3(24.0) p>0.05, HI-C 110.2(24.2) to 113.7(26.5), p>0.05). The Dichotic Listening test by Obuchi et al. [35] showed no significant difference (displayed graphically in their paper) after three years of follow-up. Dawes et al. [38] reported no difference between intervention and control groups both at 5- and 11-year follow-up in the Digit Symbol Substitution Test and Trail Making Test-A, and concluded that their study is “not supportive of a robust effect of hearing aid use as being protective against cognitive decline”. In two tests (flanker and processing speed) by Karawani et al. [31] no differences for either group (respectively HA: 109.75(10.57) to 110.99(13.35), p = 0.591, HI-C: 99.82(10.86) to 106.30(13.07), p = 0.151; HA: 96.90(19.60) to 100.48(15.23), p = 0.308, HI-C: 88.75(18.01) to 91.02(19.45), p = 0.598). Brewster et al. [32] reported no differential numerical improvements for the hearing aid group for the Attention test (np-ES = 0.16).

Three studies without control groups also assessed complex attention. Desjardins et al. [43], displaying the data of their six participants individually, showed improvement for 6/6 participants in Corpus response measure, 4/6 in the Digit Symbol Substitution Test, 3/6 in the Stroop test and 2/6 in the Auditory reaction time. The Digit Symbol Substitution Test and Trail Making Test-A showed no significant improvement after six months of hearing aid use in the study by Deal et al. [30] (40.2(10.2) to 40.8(11.5); 33(29,49.5) to 33(27.5,42). Complex attention measured by identification test [45] showed no improvement after 18 months hearing aid use (2.78(0.06) to 2.78(0.07), p = 0.869).

Language

Deal et al. [30] showed no significant improvement after six months in Word fluency and Boston naming test (33.7(12.3) to 33.6(13.8); 26.7(2.5) to 27.2(2.5). The Verbal Fluency test, in the study van Hooren et al. [34] and Dawes et al. [38] both showed no significant improvement in the hearing intervention group after 12-months and 11-years, respectively (HA: 27.30(6.63) to 25.18(6.87), HI-C 26.72(6.30) to 23.22(6.32), p = 0.22; HA: 26.2(2.3), HI-C 29.2(0.7), p = 0.21). Tesch-Römer et al. [33] showed no difference between the intervention group and the control group six months post-intervention in the Spot-a-word, Letter ‘s’ test, Naming animals (HA: 19.5(4.1) to 19.8(4.3), p>0.05, HI-C: 20.4(3.0) to 21.2(3.0), p>0.05; HA: 19.4(7.1) to 20.4(8.3) p>0.05, HI-C 21.5(7.8) to 21.5(7.1), p>0.05; HA: 26.4(7.6) to 25.9(7.6), p>0.05, HI-C: 27.2(7.3) to 28.1(8.7), p>0.05) respectively). Brewster et al. [32] demonstrated no improvement after 12 weeks of hearing aids in the Language test (np-ES = 0.06).

Learning and memory

Choi et al. [36], focusing on the cognitive domain learning and memory, found that total Visual Verbal Learning Task score and Visual Verbal Learning Task recognition score significantly improved only in the hearing aid group, respectively 32.7(8.3) to 38.5(11.8), p<0.05 and 11.7(1.9) to 13.1(1.5), p<0.05, although the VVLT latency score did not (HA: 7.5(2.9) to 8.4(2.5), p>0.05). The Visual Verbal Learning Task -immediate and–delayed performed in the study of van Hooren et al. [34] did not show this improvement (HA: 22.89(5.97) to 25.50(5.63), HI-C 23.59(4.76) to 24.96(5.85), p = 0.69; HA: 9.64(3.19) to 10.09(2.91), HI-C 8.31(2.98) to 9.47(3.44), p = 0.40). Dawes et al. [38] found that the auditory verbal learning test showed no improvement after 11 years of hearing aid use as well (HA: 3.2(0.5), HI-C: 4.1(0.1), p = 0.09).

Learning and memory assessed in studies without a control group showed no significant difference in one of the tests, Delayed word recall, incidental learning [30] and one card learning test [45] (5.6(1.6) to 6.1(1.5); 3.5(2.2) to 4.3(2.2); 0.94(0.14) to 0.96(0.11), p = 0.262, respectively). Delayed Memory and the visuospatial/constructional test in the study of Brewster et al. [32] also showed no meaningful improvements (np-ES = 0 and np-ES = 0.60, respectively).

Discussion

To study the effect of hearing aids on cognitive function in persons with hearing loss, we systematically reviewed the literature in which hearing aids were used to treat hearing loss in persons without pre-existent dementia. Seventeen studies ranging from 1990 to 2020 were included, totalling 3526 participants. Four of these were RCTs and 13 were prospective cohort studies, with the length of follow-up ranging from 6 weeks to 14 years. In this systematic review only three studies had a follow up time of longer than 12 months [35, 38, 44], namely 3, 11 and 14 years. Ten cohort studies were assessed with a high risk of bias [33, 35, 3743, 45], and three RCTs are judged to have some concerns [3032]. Concerning these limitations, in particular the short follow-up time, we find it premature to make definitive conclusions about the effect of hearing aids on cognitive function at this point.

In this systematic review, we provided an overview on outcomes per cognitive domain which showed that if beneficial effects of hearing aids on cognition exists, they particularly seem to affect the executive function domain. Additionally, using hearing aids might not have an effect on the language domain and little, if any, effect on complex attention and learning and memory.

Previous systematic reviews researching the relationship between hearing impairment and cognitive function diverge in their interpretations of the effects of hearing aids. Taljaard et al. 2015 [11] concluded that “at group level, individuals with treated hearing impairment demonstrate superior cognition to those with untreated hearing impairment,” yet they find that the measured effect decreases when adjusting for publication bias. In contrast, in the systematic review of Mamo et al. 2018 [20] the authors concluded no significant changes in cognitive function after hearing aid use, in which conclusions were based on only three studies. Also, in the systematic review performed by Thomson et al. 2017 [19] the authors described that most of their included studies demonstrated no correlation between hearing aid use and cognition. Their study included mostly studies in which hearing aid use was determined at one time point with a yes or no question. In our study, only longitudinal studies that included participants with first-time hearing aid use were eligible. This gave more insight whether hearing aids might be preventive for cognitive decline. Our results resonate with these previous findings as we do not find a definitive answer to whether hearing aids are beneficial for cognition as a whole. However, our review does offer a new insight as we use a different perspective by not only examining the effect of hearing aids on cognition as a whole, but by examining the outcomes per cognitive domain.

It is difficult to precisely measure cognitive ability, with cognition being such a broad concept. There are many different tests available, which measure different aspects of cognition. This is reflected in the large variety of outcome measures used in the studies included in this review. There is, however, a danger to this as the selection of the tests heavily influences the results. Remarkably, many studies did not justify the reasons for the use of their tests. Furthermore, many of the tests are designed for a normal hearing population and involve either spoken instruction or involve hearing and reacting to a stimulus. The validity of these tests in a population with hearing impairment is questionable and cognitive tests relying on auditory stimuli could potentially overestimate the effects of hearing aids on cognition.

The mechanism underlying the association between hearing loss and cognitive impairment is not well understood, yet it is important to elucidate the relationship to either minimize its impact or develop preventative and rehabilitative measures. Three recent articles [2, 46, 47] describe an in-depth review of these hypotheses. These theories will only be briefly discussed in relation to the results of hearing aid treatment presented in this paper. The ‘common-cause hypothesis’ states that both cognitive and hearing decline are related to a common neurodegenerative process effecting both the aging brain and auditory system [2, 46]. The ‘sensory-deprivation hypothesis’ postulates that sensory decline causes more long-term cognitive decline. Hearing rehabilitation might reduce the risk of dementia, but the improvement or acute cessation of cognitive decline is not expected [46]. For the common cause theory, over time cognitive function should continue to decline, regardless of the use of hearing aids [2, 46]. For the majority of cognitive outcome measures, there was no significant effect of intervention. It is possible that the lack of significant benefit on the majority of cognitive domains support these hypotheses.

Conversely, the ‘cascade hypothesis’ states that, impoverished sensory input can lead to cognitive impairment directly or indirectly through decreased stimulation of cognitive processing [2, 47]. Restoration of hearing with hearing aids can cease the cognitive decline [2]. Additionally, the ‘cognitive load hypothesis’ posits that hearing loss leads to degraded auditory signals, which in turn requires greater cognitive resources for auditory processing [2, 3, 8]. This hypothesis describes a situation for hearing-impaired people which is similar to performing ‘dual tasks’ at the same time, resulting in cognitive reserve depletion and limiting working memory. This excessive cognitive load eventually leads to structural changes and neurodegeneration [46]. Interestingly, Griffiths et al. [47] have recently introduced an alternative hypothesis: ‘the role of the medial temporal lobe’. Similar to the cognitive load hypothesis, hearing loss alters cortical activity, particularly in the medial temporal lobe. However, the critical difference is the interaction between the altered activity with dementia pathology. Use of hearing aids would not restore cognitive function in this case [47]. The information-degradation hypothesis postulates that cognitive function declines in older adults results as a consequence of compensating for diminished auditory input [46]. Difficulties in perception cascade ‘upwards,’ which in turn compromises higher level of cognitive processing because resources have been used for auditory perception. More effortful listening places more demand on executive function and working memory [46]. The results from our review are partly consistent with these hypotheses, showing that if a beneficial effect would exist, we would expect it to affect the specific cognitive domain of executive function.

Arguments with varying degrees of strength can be made for each hypothesis and to date none of the discussed mechanisms can be ruled out [47]. Ultimately, even though the hypotheses are presented as alternatives, it is probable that they are not mutually exclusive [48]. Johnson et al. [49] further highlights the potential importance of the auditory brain, away from the oversimplification of the dichotomy of brain and ear. The neurodegeneration of central hearing (auditory processing mechanisms) will amplify degrees of peripheral hearing loss. Potentially hearing loss is an early warning sign for dementia [49]. Considering this, we can question whether pure tone average testing is the right way to measure hearing loss in persons with cognitive decline, and whether or not hearing aids addressing peripheral hearing can address the link between hearing loss and cognitive decline. Importantly, a better understanding of the underlying mechanisms will allow for a better understanding of how realistic prevention effort may be. Furthermore, a better understanding would also lead to more accurate and consistent choices in cognitive tests, and give clearer interpretations to their results.

The overall poor quality of the included studies, in particular due to a short follow-up time, is a major limitation of our study. Bearing in mind that cognitive decline is a slowly progressing impairment, it is inadequate to draw conclusions about the effect of an intervention on cognitive function based on the presented results [3]. In this review, we included two studies with follow-up longer than 10 years; Dawes et al. [38], 11 years, and Maharani et al. [44], 14 years. The former [38] reports no significant difference between intervention and control for all outcomes measured. The latter [44] showed that although episodic memory declined with age; this rate of decline was significantly less after the start of hearing aids. It should be noted that in the study by Dawes et al. [38] there was a significant mean hearing level difference between hearing aid users and the control group (38.9dB and 29.8dB, respectively). Furthermore, the process of cognitive decline is a heterogenous, multidimensional phenomenon, and yet the large majority of the studies do not satisfactorily adjust for important confounders, such as daily activities or education level [3]. Next to this, as can be seen in the risk of bias assessments, in many studies there is no or little ascertainment of exposure. In several studies hearing aid compliance was either poorly [29, 33, 38, 44] or not described [35, 36, 39, 40, 42]. This can affect the results drastically, as the treatment group might underutilize the hearing aids and therefore did not gain significant exposure. It is also difficult to blind either participant or researcher for the intervention, which adds another source of potential bias.

Regarding the eight studies that compared intervention to control groups, it is important to note that four studies [33, 35, 36, 38] had intervention groups with significantly worse hearing loss than the controls. Only one of these studies [36] showed a positive significant effect of intervention on cognitive outcome. Taljaard et al. [11] have described a dose-response relationship between hearing loss and cognitive function, with Golub et al. [50] finding a decrease in cognition with every 10dB reduction in hearing. Considering this knowledge, potentially more of an effect would have been seen if both control group and intervention group had similar hearing loss levels.

Dementia is a collective noun for many diseases that affect cognition. These specific diseases, such as Alzheimer’s Disease, Vascular Dementia, Dementia with Lewy Bodies or Fronto-temporal Dementia, all have different aetiologies [51]. In the included articles the authors do not differentiate between the different sub-types. For the scope of this review, cognitive decline is defined as a heterogeneous clinical syndrome. For the future it would be interesting to evaluate the different subtypes of dementia and the link to their underlying pathophysiology’s.

Considering hearing loss is considered to be one of the largest modifiable risk factors for developing dementia [5, 6], it is essential to study the effect of intervention on populations before they reach the stage of dementia. One of the strengths of our review is strict inclusion criteria, in which only participants without pre-existent dementia were included. This allows us to review the intervention effects of hearing aids on cognitive decline. Substantial heterogeneity remained, however. Furthermore, due to the large variety of cognitive tests and lengths of follow-up, the value in purely comparing the included studies is limited. It was for this reason that we divided the cognitive outcome measures into the seven broad cognitive domains to permit comparison. Not only does this give insight into potential domains involved in association of hearing loss, cognitive function and the effect of hearing aids, but can also provide the foundation for future studies considering which cognitive test measures to include.

Clinical applications and further research

A key message from the Lancet reports [3, 5] is to be ambitious in terms of prevention, after all ‘prevention is better than cure.’ Considering that both hearing loss and dementia are separately big public health problems, it would be folly not to treat for hearing loss. Additionally, depression, social isolation and physical inactivity are considered separate modifiable risk factors for dementia [3, 5] and seeing as hearing loss also affects physical and mental health [7, 912], treatment with hearing aids could have beneficial effects in multiple dimensions. Fundamentally, hearing aids are primarily used to benefit communication and social functioning. Additionally, hearing aids are known to positively affect mood and quality of life [39, 52]. Despite this, close to four-fifths of persons with hearing loss do not use hearing aids [14]. These low rates might be caused by the stigmatization, underestimation of the benefits of hearing rehabilitation and the lack of access [53]. Seeing as hearing loss is easily diagnosable and treatable, we advise clinicians to be vigilant for hearing loss and to counsel patients on the benefits of hearing aids.

Regarding future research, firstly, there is a need for long-term, large-scale observational studies to better understand the effects of hearing aids. Cognitive decline can only be adequately observed in studies that span multiple years, if not decades [48]. Ideally, this would be researched in the form of a randomized clinical trial. On this note, the ACHIEVE trial is currently ongoing, a trial of 850 older adults randomized to either a hearing intervention or successful aging intervention, being an interactive health education program, to study the effects on cognition [30, 54]. Secondly, as is clear from the wide variety of cognitive measures discussed in this review, there is a need for a standardization of well-justified cognitive measures to use for this purpose. Thirdly, further elucidation of the underlying mechanisms is necessary, and an important step for further research is the stratification of results according to subtypes of dementia. Fourthly, even though domains of social and emotional cognition have not been focused on in wider literature, it would be relevant to evaluate the effects of hearing aids on these as well. Finally, as the 2020 Lancet Commission on dementia prevention, intervention, and care recognizes hearing loss to be a mid-life risk factor for dementia and considering hearing loss starts around mid-life, it would be interesting to conduct studies in slightly younger adults [5].

Conclusion

The included studies in this systematic review had a high risk of bias, in particular due to a short time of follow up. Therefore, definitive conclusions about the effects of hearing aids on cognition or the prevention of cognitive decline are difficult to draw. The precise effect of hearing aids on cognitive function is debatable.

Supporting information

S2 File. PROSPERO international prospective register of systematic reviews.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0261207.s002

(PDF)

References

  1. 1. Patterson C. World Alzheimer report 2018. London Alzheimer’s Dis Int 2018.
  2. 2. Uchida Y, Sugiura S, Nishita Y, Saji N, Sone M, Ueda H. Age-related hearing loss and cognitive decline—The potential mechanisms linking the two. Auris Nasus Larynx. 2019 Feb;46(1):1–9. pmid:30177417
  3. 3. Livingston G, Sommerlad A, Orgeta V, Costafreda SG, Huntley J, Ames D, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet. 2017 Dec 16;390(10113):2673–2734. pmid:28735855
  4. 4. Berchtold NC, Cotman CW. Evolution in the conceptualization of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: Greco-Roman period to the 1960s. Neurobiol Aging. 1998 May 1;19(3):173–89. pmid:9661992
  5. 5. Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet. 2020 Aug 8;396(10248):413–446. pmid:32738937
  6. 6. World Health Organization. Factsheet: Deafness and hearing loss [Internet]. [place, publisher unknown] 1 Mar 2020. [cited 2020 Nov 11]. https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/deafness-and-hearing-loss
  7. 7. James SL, Abate D, Abate KH, Abay SM, Abbafati C, Abbasi N, et al. Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 354 Diseases and Injuries for 195 countries and territories, 1990–2017: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2017. Lancet. 2017 Nov 10;392(10159):1789–858.
  8. 8. Loughrey DG, Kelly ME, Kelley GA, Brennan S, Lawlor BA. Association of Age-Related Hearing Loss With Cognitive Function, Cognitive Impairment, and Dementia: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2018 Feb 1;144(2):115–126. Erratum in: JAMA Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2018 Feb 1;144(2):176. pmid:29222544
  9. 9. Pronk M, Deeg DJ, Kramer SE. Hearing status in older persons: a significant determinant of depression and loneliness? Results from the longitudinal aging study amsterdam. Am J Audiol. 2013 Dec;22(2):316–20. pmid:24018571
  10. 10. Lawrence BJ, Jayakody DMP, Bennett RJ, Eikelboom RH, Gasson N, Friedland PL. Hearing Loss and Depression in Older Adults: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Gerontologist. 2020 Apr 2;60(3):e137–e154. pmid:30835787
  11. 11. Taljaard DS, Olaithe M, Brennan-Jones CG, Eikelboom RH, Bucks RS. The relationship between hearing impairment and cognitive function: a meta-analysis in adults. Clin Otolaryngol. 2016 Dec;41(6):718–729. pmid:26670203
  12. 12. Hogan A, O’Loughlin K, Miller P, Kendig H. The health impact of a hearing disability on older people in Australia. J Aging Health. 2009 Dec;21(8):1098–111 pmid:19897778
  13. 13. Huang CQ, Dong BR, Lu ZC, Yue JR, Liu QX. Chronic diseases and risk for depression in old age: a meta-analysis of published literature. Ageing Res Rev. 2010 Apr;9(2):131–41 pmid:19524072
  14. 14. Chien W, Lin FR. Prevalence of hearing aid use among older adults in the United States. Arch Intern Med. 2012 Feb 13;172(3):292–3. pmid:22332170
  15. 15. Peracino A. Hearing loss and dementia in the aging population. Audiol Neurootol. 2014;19 Suppl 1:6–9 pmid:25733359
  16. 16. Lin FR, Metter EJ, O’Brien RJ, Resnick SM, Zonderman AB, Ferrucci L. Hearing loss and incident dementia. Arch Neurol. 2011 Feb;68(2):214–20. pmid:21320988
  17. 17. Lin FR, Yaffe K, Xia J, Xue QL, Harris TB, Purchase-Helzner E, et al. Hearing loss and cognitive decline in older adults. JAMA Intern Med. 2013 Feb 25;173(4):293–9. pmid:23337978
  18. 18. Deal JA, Sharrett AR, Albert MS, Coresh J, Mosley TH, Knopman D, et al. Hearing impairment and cognitive decline: a pilot study conducted within the atherosclerosis risk in communities neurocognitive study. Am J Epidemiol. 2015 May 1;181(9):680–90 pmid:25841870
  19. 19. Thomson RS, Auduong P, Miller AT, Gurgel RK. Hearing loss as a risk factor for dementia: A systematic review. Laryngoscope Investig Otolaryngol. 2017 Mar 16;2(2):69–79. pmid:28894825
  20. 20. Mamo SK, Reed NS, Price C, Occhipinti D, Pletnikova A, Lin FR, et al. Hearing Loss Treatment in Older Adults With Cognitive Impairment: A Systematic Review. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2018 Oct 26;61(10):2589–2603. pmid:30304320
  21. 21. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG; PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. BMJ. 2009 Jul 21;339:b2535. pmid:19622551
  22. 22. Sanders M.E., Smit A.L., Stegeman I. The effects of hearing aids on cognitive function: a systematic review protocol. PROSPERO CRD42020171872. 2020.
  23. 23. ClinicalTrials.gov [cited 2020 Feb 12] [Internet] https://www.clinicaltrials.gov/
  24. 24. Netherlands Trial Register (NTR) [cited 2020 Feb 12]. [Internet]. https://www.trialregister.nl/
  25. 25. Roger S. McIntyre, Danielle S. Cha, Joanna K. Soczynska. Cognition in Major Depressive Disorder. Oxford University Press; 2014
  26. 26. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. Washington; 2013.
  27. 27. Higgins JP, Altman DG, Gøtzsche PC, Jüni P, Moher D, Oxman AD, et al. Cochrane Bias Methods Group; Cochrane Statistical Methods Group. The Cochrane Collaboration’s tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ. 2011 Oct 18;343:d5928. pmid:22008217
  28. 28. Wells G, Shea B, O’Connell D, Peterson je, Welch V, Losos M, et al. The Newcastle–Ottawa Scale (NOS) for Assessing the Quality of Non-Randomized Studies in Meta-Analysis. 2000 Jan 1
  29. 29. Mulrow CD, Aguilar C, Endicott JE, Tuley MR, Velez R, Charlip WS, et al. Quality-of-life changes and hearing impairment. A randomized trial. Ann Intern Med. 1990 Aug 1;113(3):188–94. pmid:2197909
  30. 30. Deal JA, Albert MS, Arnold M, Bangdiwala SI, Chisolm T, Davis S, et al. A randomized feasibility pilot trial of hearing treatment for reducing cognitive decline: Results from the Aging and Cognitive Health Evaluation in Elders Pilot Study. Alzheimers Dement (N Y). 2017 Jun 21;3(3):410–415. pmid:29067347
  31. 31. Karawani H, Jenkins K, Anderson S. Restoration of sensory input may improve cognitive and neural function. Neuropsychologia. 2018 Jun 1;114:203–13. pmid:29729278
  32. 32. Brewster KK, Pavlicova M, Stein A, Chen M, Chen C, Brown PJ, et al. A pilot randomized controlled trial of hearing aids to improve mood and cognition in older adults. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2020 Aug;35(8):842–850. pmid:32291802
  33. 33. Tesch-Römer C. Psychological effects of hearing aid use in older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1997 May;52(3):127–38. pmid:9158564
  34. 34. van Hooren SA, Anteunis LJ, Valentijn SA, Bosma H, Ponds RW, Jolles J, et al. Does cognitive function in older adults with hearing impairment improve by hearing aid use? Int J Audiol. 2005 May;44(5):265–71. pmid:16028789
  35. 35. Obuchi C, Harashima T, Shiroma M. Age-Related Changes in Auditory and Cognitive Abilities in Elderly Persons with Hearing Aids Fitted at the Initial Stages of Hearing Loss. Audiol Res. 2011 Mar 9;1(1):e11. pmid:26557295
  36. 36. Choi AY, Shim HJ, Lee SH, Yoon SW, Joo EJ. Is cognitive function in adults with hearing impairment improved by the use of hearing AIDS? Clin Exp Otorhinolaryngol. 2011 Jun;4(2):72–6. pmid:21716953
  37. 37. Doherty KA, Desjardins JL. The benefit of amplification on auditory working memory function in middle-aged and young-older hearing impaired adults. Front Psychol. 2015 Jun 5;6:721. pmid:26097461
  38. 38. Dawes P, Cruickshanks KJ, Fischer ME, Klein BE, Klein R, Nondahl DM. Hearing-aid use and long-term health outcomes: Hearing handicap, mental health, social engagement, cognitive function, physical health, and mortality. Int J Audiol. 2015;54(11):838–44. pmid:26140300
  39. 39. Acar B, Yurekli MF, Babademez MA, Karabulut H, Karasen RM. Effects of hearing aids on cognitive functions and depressive signs in elderly people. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2011 May 1;52(3):250–2. pmid:20472312
  40. 40. Magalhães R, Iório MC. Evaluation of participation restriction and cognitive processes in the elderly before and after the audiologic rehabilitation. J Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2011 Mar;23(1):51–6. pmid:21552733
  41. 41. Boi R, Racca L, Cavallero A, Carpaneto V, Racca M, Dall’ Acqua F, et al. Hearing loss and depressive symptoms in elderly patients. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2012 Jul;12(3):440–5. pmid:22212622
  42. 42. Castiglione A, Benatti A, Velardita C, Favaro D, Padoan E, Severi D, et al. Aging, Cognitive Decline and Hearing Loss: Effects of Auditory Rehabilitation and Training with Hearing Aids and Cochlear Implants on Cognitive Function and Depression among Older Adults. Audiol Neurootol. 2016;21 Suppl 1:21–28.
  43. 43. Desjardins JL. Analysis of Performance on Cognitive Test Measures Before, During, and After 6 Months of Hearing Aid Use: A Single-Subject Experimental Design. Am J Audiol. 2016 Jun 1;25(2):127–41. pmid:27249016
  44. 44. Maharani A, Dawes P, Nazroo J, Tampubolon G, Pendleton N; SENSE-Cog WP1 group. Longitudinal Relationship Between Hearing Aid Use and Cognitive Function in Older Americans. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2018 Jul;66(6):1130–1136. Erratum in: J Am Geriatr Soc. 2018 Dec;66(12):2435. pmid:29637544
  45. 45. Sarant J, Harris D, Busby P, Maruff P, Schembri A, Lemke U, et al. The Effect of Hearing Aid Use on Cognition in Older Adults: Can We Delay Decline or Even Improve Cognitive Function? J Clin Med. 2020 Jan 17;9(1):254. pmid:31963547
  46. 46. Wayne RV, Johnsrude IS. A review of causal mechanisms underlying the link between age-related hearing loss and cognitive decline. Ageing Res Rev. 2015 Sep;23(Pt B):154–66. pmid:26123097
  47. 47. Griffiths TD, Lad M, Kumar S, Holmes E, McMurray B, Maguire EA, et al. How Can Hearing Loss Cause Dementia? Neuron. 2020 Nov 11;108(3):401–412. pmid:32871106
  48. 48. Amieva H, Ouvrard C. Does Treating Hearing Loss in Older Adults Improve Cognitive Outcomes? A Review. J Clin Med. 2020 Mar 16;9(3):805. pmid:32188074
  49. 49. Johnson JCS, Marshall CR, Weil RS, Bamiou DE, Hardy CJD, Warren JD. Hearing and dementia: from ears to brain. Brain. 2021 Mar 3;144(2):391–401. pmid:33351095
  50. 50. Golub JS, Brickman AM, Ciarleglio AJ, Schupf N, Luchsinger JA. Association of subclinical hearing loss with cognitive performance. JAMA Otolaryngol—Head Neck Surg. 2020 Jan 1;146(1):57–67. pmid:31725853
  51. 51. Hugo J, Ganguli M. Dementia and cognitive impairment: epidemiology, diagnosis, and treatment. Clin Geriatr Med. 2014 Aug;30(3):421–42. pmid:25037289
  52. 52. Yamada Y, Švejdíková B, Kisvetrová H. Improvement of older-person-specific QOL after hearing aid fitting and its relation to social interaction. J Commun Disord. 2017 May;67:14–21 pmid:28505472
  53. 53. Meyer C, Hickson L. What factors influence help-seeking for hearing impairment and hearing aid adoption in older adults? Int J Audiol. 2012;51(2):66–74. pmid:22256793
  54. 54. Sanchez VA, Arnold ML, Reed NS, Oree PH, Matthews CR, Clock Eddins A, et al. The Hearing Intervention for the Aging and Cognitive Health Evaluation in Elders Randomized Control Trial: Manualization and Feasibility Study. Ear Hear. 2020 Sep/Oct;41(5):1333–1348. pmid:32251012