skip to main content
research-article

The impact of interface affordances on human ideation, problem solving, and inferential reasoning

Published:25 October 2012Publication History
Skip Abstract Section

Abstract

This article presents two studies investigating how computer interface affordances influence basic cognition, including ideational fluency, problem solving, and inferential reasoning. In one study comparing interfaces with different input capabilities, students expressed 56% more nonlinguistic representations (diagrams, symbols, numbers) when using pen interfaces. A linear regression confirmed that nonlinguistic communication directly mediated a substantial increase (38.5%) in students' ability to produce appropriate science ideas. In contrast, students expressed 41% more linguistic content when using a keyboard-based interface, which mediated a drop in science ideation. A follow-up study pursued the question of how interfaces that prime nonlinguistic communication so effectively facilitate cognition. This study examined the relation between students' expression of nonlinguistic representations and their inference accuracy when using analogous digital and non-digital pen tools. Perhaps surprisingly, the digital pen interface stimulated construction of more diagrams, more correct Venn diagrams, and more accurate domain inferences. Students' construction of multiple diagrams to represent a problem also directly suppressed overgeneralization errors, which were the most common inference failure. These research results reveal that computer interfaces have communications affordances which elicit communication patterns that can substantially stimulate or impede basic cognition. Implications are discussed for designing new digital tools for thinking, with an emphasis on nonlinguistic and especially spatial representations that are most poorly supported by current keyboard-based interfaces.

References

  1. ANOTO. 2011. http://www.anoto.com/. (Last accessed 10/11).Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  2. Bangert-Drowns, R. 1993. The word processor as an instructional tool: A meta-analysis of word processing in writing instruction. Rev. Edu. Res. 63, 69--93.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  3. Barrows, H., Norman, G., Neufeld, V., and Feightner, J. 1982. The clinical reasoning of randomly selected physicians in general medical practice. Clin. Invest. Med. 5, 49--55.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  4. Bauer, M. and Johnson-Laird, P. 1993. How diagrams can improve reasoning. Psychol. Sci. 4, 6, 372--378.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  5. Bloom, P., Peterson, M., Nadel, L., and Garrett, M., Eds. 1996. Language and Space. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  6. Crowne, S. 2007. Harnessing Technology Review 2007: Progress and Impact of Technology on Education (BEC1-15506). Coventry: Becta Reviews.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  7. Darves, C. and Oviatt, S. 2004. Talking to digital fish: Designing effective conversational interfaces for educational software. In Evaluating Embodied Conversational Agents, Z. Ruttkay, C. Pelachaud, Eds. Kluwer, Dordrecht, 7, 271--292. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  8. Gibson, J. 1977. The theory of affordances. In Perceiving, Acting and Knowing, R. Shaw, J. Bransford, Eds. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 3, 67--82.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  9. Guilford, J. P. 1956. The structure of intellect. Psychol. Bull. 53, 267--293.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  10. Haas, C. 1989. Does the medium make the difference? Two studies of writing with pen and paper and with computers. Hum. Comput. Interac. 4, 149--169. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  11. Johnson-Laird, P. 1999. Space to think. In Language and Space, P. Bloom, M. Peterson, L. Nadel, and M. Garrett, Eds. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 437--462.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  12. Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., and Tversky, A., Eds. 1982. Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Cambridge University Press, NY.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  13. Kirk, R. 1995. Experimental Design: Procedures for the Behavioral Sciences, 3rd Ed., Brooks/Cole Publishing. Pacific Grove, CA.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  14. Larkin, J. and Simon, H. 1987. Why a diagram is (sometimes) worth ten thousand words. Cognit. Sci. 11, 65--99.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  15. LIVESCRIBE. 2011. Livescribe home page. http://www.livescribe.com. (Last accessed 10/11).Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  16. Levinson, S. 2003. Space in Language and Cognition: Explorations in Cognitive Diversity, Language, Culture and Cognition. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, U.K.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  17. Luria, A. 1961. The Role of Speech in the Regulation Normal and Abnormal Behavior. Liveright, New York, NY.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  18. ONENOTE. 2011. Microsoft One Note. http://www.microsoft.com/office/onenote/. (Last accessed 10/11).Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  19. Oviatt, S. To appear. The Future of Educational Interfaces. Routledge Press, London, U.K.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  20. Oviatt, S., Arthur, A., Brock, Y. and Cohen, J. 2007. Expressive pen-based interfaces for math education. In Proceedings of the Conference on Computer Supported Collaborative Learning 2007: Of Mice, Minds and Society, International Society of the Learning Sciences, C. Chinn, G. Erkens S. Puntambekar, Eds. Vol. 8, Part 2, 569--578. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  21. Oviatt, S., Arthur, A., and Cohen, J. 2006. Quiet interfaces that help students think. In Proceedings of the 19th Annual ACM Symposium on User Interface Software and Technology, 191--200. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  22. Oviatt, S. and Cohen, A. 2010a. Toward high-performance communications interfaces for science problem-solving. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 19, 6, 515--531.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  23. Oviatt, S. and Cohen, A. 2010b. Supporting students' thinking marks: Designing accessible interfaces for science education. In Proceedings of the American Educational Research Association Conference.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  24. Roth, W. M. 2005. Talking Science: Language and Learning in Science Classrooms. Rowman and Littlefield, Toronto.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  25. Ruths, D., Nakhleh, L. Ivengar, M., Reddy, S., and Ram, P. 2006. Graph-theoretic hypothesis generation in biological signaling networks. J. Comput. Biol. 13, 1546--1557.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  26. Schwartz, D. and Heiser, J. 2006. Spatial representations and imagery in learning. In Cambridge Handbook of the Learning Sciences, R. Sawyer, Ed. Cambridge University Press, New York, NY, 19, 283--298.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  27. Smith, M., Wood, W., Adams, W., Wieman, C., Knight, J., Guild, N., and Su, T. 2009. Why peer discussion improves student performance on in-class concept questions. Science 323, 122--124.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  28. Stieff, M. and Raje, S. 2010. Expert algorithmic and imagistic problem solving strategies in advanced chemistry. Spatial Cogn. Comput. 10, 53--81.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  29. Suthers, D. and Hundhausen, C. 2003. An experimental study of the effects of representational guidance on collaborative learning. J. Learn. Sci. 12, 183--219.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  30. Sweller, J., van Merrienboer, J., and Paas, F. 1998. Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Edu. Psychol. Rev. 10, 251--257.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  31. Tversky, B. and Suwa, M. 2009. Thinking with sketches. In Tools for Innovation, A. Markman, Oxford University Press, Oxford, U.K. 75--84.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  32. Vvygotsky, L. 1962. Thought and Language. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  33. Weber, E., Bockenholt, U., Hilton, D., and Wallace, B. 1993. Determinants of diagnostic hypothesis generation. Effects on information, base rates, and experience. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Memory Cognit. 19, 1151--1164.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  34. van Merrienboer, J. and Sweller, J. 2005. Cognitive load theory and complex learning: Recent developments and future directions. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 17, 2, 147--177.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  35. Zhang, H. and Linn, M. 2008. Using drawings to support learning from dynamic visualizations. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association (AERA), New York, NY.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar

Index Terms

  1. The impact of interface affordances on human ideation, problem solving, and inferential reasoning

        Recommendations

        Comments

        Login options

        Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.

        Sign in

        Full Access

        • Published in

          cover image ACM Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction
          ACM Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction  Volume 19, Issue 3
          October 2012
          209 pages
          ISSN:1073-0516
          EISSN:1557-7325
          DOI:10.1145/2362364
          Issue’s Table of Contents

          Copyright © 2012 ACM

          Permission to make digital or hard copies of all or part of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than ACM must be honored. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, or republish, to post on servers or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee. Request permissions from [email protected]

          Publisher

          Association for Computing Machinery

          New York, NY, United States

          Publication History

          • Published: 25 October 2012
          • Accepted: 1 June 2012
          • Revised: 1 November 2011
          • Received: 1 January 2011
          Published in tochi Volume 19, Issue 3

          Permissions

          Request permissions about this article.

          Request Permissions

          Check for updates

          Qualifiers

          • research-article
          • Research
          • Refereed

        PDF Format

        View or Download as a PDF file.

        PDF

        eReader

        View online with eReader.

        eReader