The nature of playfulness in young adults

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.02.018Get rights and content

Abstract

Playful people are uniquely able to transform virtually any environment to make it more stimulating, enjoyable and entertaining. Efforts to more systematically characterize the playful person have proven more fruitful with children than with adults. To determine if playfulness could be identified as a meaningful psychological construct in adults, focus groups of undergraduate students first described characteristics of highly playful and nonplayful people. Six hundred and forty-nine students rated themselves on playfulness and on these descriptors, and then identified and rated others high and low in playfulness. Fifteen qualities were found to uniquely describe a playful individual, resulting in four component qualities of “gregarious”, “uninhibited”, “comedic” and “dynamic” for both men and women. Participants defined playfulness and its inherent qualities in the same way whether they perceived it in themselves or in others.

Introduction

Both lay and empirical observations have long noted that certain individuals appear able to transform virtually any environment to make it (more) enjoyable and entertaining – terming them “playful” individuals. However, efforts to more systematically characterize the playful person and to capture these apparent playful qualities have been more successful with children than with adults (Barnett, 1990, Barnett, 1991a, Barnett, 1991b, Barnett and Kleiber, 1982, Barnett and Kleiber, 1984, Lieberman, 1965, Lieberman, 1966, Rogers et al., 1987). Lieberman (1977) sought to extend her original work with children to adolescents and then to adults, but with less success as the age of the player increased (see also Tegano, 1990). Glynn, 1991, Glynn and Webster, 1992, Glynn and Webster, 1993 attempted to characterize adult playfulness by asking college students and child care workers to provide descriptors that would distinguish high and low playfulness in the workplace (Glynn, 1991). They found five factors that they labeled “spontaneity”, “expressiveness”, “fun”, “creative”, and “silly” (Glynn & Webster, 1992). Their conceptualization of playfulness, however, was based on the assumption that play is the opposite of work – an assumption that has been refuted by theoreticians and empiricists alike (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990, Csikszentmihalyi and LeFevre, 1989, Starbuck and Webster, 1991). In addition, a number of other problems have been found with the content and construct validity of the scale in capturing the playfulness construct (Kruger, 1995).

The goal of this study was to determine if playfulness could be more precisely identified as a meaningful psychological construct in young adults (university students). This was approached by asking lay persons to provide content based on their natural language and experiences, and by correlating playfulness ratings with personality descriptors, taken both on oneself and on others.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants were 649 undergraduate students recruited from social and behavioral science classes at three large midwestern universities, with extra credit being offered for participation. The sample ranged in age from 18 to 30 years (mean age = 22.45); 46% were male and 54% were female.

Measures

Utilizing a focus group format, small groups of 6–10 student volunteers were asked what characteristics came to mind when someone is thought of as being a playful individual. They were also asked to talk about

Results

Initial analyses indicated no differences between the classes or universities on any of the measures in this study (all p > .05), thus, these distinctions were removed in all subsequent analyses.

Discussion and conclusion

In the present study, focus group members and study participants were able to easily recognize the “playfulness” term, and conjure up images and descriptions of who a playful person might be, thus attesting to the ecological validity of the construct. This study identified 15 descriptive qualities of the playful individual, consistently found for both males and females, and also when assessments of playfulness were conducted on one’s self as well as on one’s friends. On each of these

References (26)

  • N. Bozionelos et al.

    Playfulness: Its relationship with instrumental and expressive traits

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (1999)
  • W.H. Starbuck et al.

    When is play productive?

    Accounting, Management, and Information Technology

    (1991)
  • L.A. Barnett

    Playfulness: Definition, design, and measurement

    Play and Culture

    (1990)
  • L.A. Barnett

    Characterizing playfulness: Correlates with individual attributes and personality traits

    Play and Culture

    (1991)
  • L.A. Barnett

    The playful child: Measurement of a disposition to play

    Play and Culture

    (1991)
  • L.A. Barnett et al.

    Concomitants of playfulness in early childhood: Cognitive abilities and gender

    Journal of Genetic Psychology

    (1982)
  • L.A. Barnett et al.

    Playfulness and the early play environment

    Journal of Genetic Psychology

    (1984)
  • R.B. Cattell

    Personality and motivation structure and measurements

    (1957)
  • M. Csikszentmihalyi

    Flow: The psychology of optimal experience

    (1990)
  • M. Csikszentmihalyi et al.

    Optimal experience in work and leisure

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1989)
  • Glynn, M. A. (1991). Framing tasks: the effects of the work and play frames on task attitudes, behavior and in...
  • M.A. Glynn et al.

    The adult playfulness scale: An initial assessment

    Psychological Reports

    (1992)
  • M.A. Glynn et al.

    Refining the nomological net of the Adult Playfulness Scale: Personality, motivational and attitudinal correlates for highly intelligent adults

    Psychological Reports

    (1993)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text